Thinking outside of the box II: Disrupting the cognitive map
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. Vallortigara,et al. From natural geometry to spatial cognition , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[2] C. Gallistel,et al. The neuroscience of learning: beyond the Hebbian synapse. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.
[3] Peter M. Jones,et al. Potentiation, overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on the shape of the environment. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[4] J. O’Keefe,et al. Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons , 1996, Nature.
[5] Laurie L Bloomfield,et al. Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry , 2005, Biology Letters.
[6] Joseph M. Austen,et al. Transfer of spatial search between environments in human adults and young children (Homo sapiens): implications for representation of local geometry by spatial systems. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.
[7] J. Pearce,et al. Absence of Overshadowing and Blocking between Landmarks and the Geometric Cues Provided by the Shape of a Test Arena , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[8] R. Rescorla. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .
[9] N. Burgess,et al. Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[10] Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al. Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: A comparison with rats , 1999 .
[11] J. Pearce,et al. Theories of associative learning in animals. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.
[12] E. Spelke,et al. Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .
[13] T. Regier,et al. An adaptive cue combination model of human spatial reorientation , 2017, Cognition.
[14] Thomas J. Wills,et al. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry , 2002, Nature.
[15] N. Burgess,et al. The Cognitive Architecture of Spatial Navigation: Hippocampal and Striatal Contributions , 2015, Neuron.
[16] N. Burgess. Spatial Cognition and the Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[17] Peter M. Jones,et al. Transfer of spatial behavior between different environments: implications for theories of spatial learning and for the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[18] Nora S. Newcombe,et al. Five Reasons to Doubt the Existence of a Geometric Module , 2010, Cogn. Sci..
[19] Peter M. Jones,et al. Further evidence that rats rely on local rather than global spatial information to locate a hidden goal: reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[20] J. H. Steiger,et al. Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. , 2004, Psychological methods.
[21] E S Spelke,et al. Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: a comparison with rats. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.
[22] Tom Beesley,et al. When goals conflict with values: counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[23] Nora S Newcombe,et al. Reorienting When Cues Conflict , 2008, Psychological science.
[24] Christian F. Doeller,et al. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[25] Mark Haselgrove,et al. Learned predictiveness training modulates biases towards using boundary or landmark cues during navigation , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[26] Matthew G. Buckley,et al. Shape Shifting: Local Landmarks Interfere With Navigation by, and Recognition of, Global Shape , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[27] Yutaka Kosaki,et al. Overshadowing of geometry learning by discrete landmarks in the water maze: effects of relative salience and relative validity of competing cues. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[28] M. Moser,et al. Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex , 2008, Science.
[29] M. Gluck,et al. Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.
[30] Joseph M. Austen,et al. Within-compound associations explain potentiation and failure to overshadow learning based on geometry by discrete landmarks. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[31] J. Pearce,et al. Failure of a landmark to restrict spatial learning based on the shape of the environment , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[32] Joseph M. Austen,et al. Dorsolateral striatal lesions impair navigation based on landmark-goal vectors but facilitate spatial learning based on a “cognitive map” , 2015, Learning & memory.
[33] N. Mackintosh. A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .
[34] J. O'Keefe,et al. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.
[35] E. Fischer. Conditioned Reflexes , 1942, American journal of physical medicine.
[36] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Toddlers' Representations of Space , 2005, Psychological science.
[37] N. Mackintosh,et al. Intradimensional and extradimensional shifts in spatial learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[38] Matthew G. Buckley,et al. Blocking Spatial Navigation Across Environments That Have a Different Shape , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[39] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Core systems of geometry in animal minds , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[40] Sara J Shettleworth,et al. The geometric module in the rat: independence of shape and feature learning in a food finding task , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[41] N. Newcombe,et al. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[42] Morgan D. Barense,et al. Boundaries Shape Cognitive Representations of Spaces and Events , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] C. Gallistel. The organization of learning , 1990 .
[44] C R Gallistel,et al. Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi & Polli (2004). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[45] E. Redhead,et al. Evidence of blocking with geometric cues in a virtual watermaze , 2009 .
[46] R. F. Wang,et al. Comparative approaches to human navigation , 2003 .
[47] R. Poldrack,et al. Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[48] J. Pearce,et al. A landmark blocks searching for a hidden platform in an environment with a distinctive shape after extended pretraining , 2009, Learning & behavior.
[49] S. Shettleworth,et al. Associative models of instrumental learning: a response to Dupuis and Dawson. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[50] R. Passingham. The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.
[51] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[52] K. Cheng. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.
[53] S. Shettleworth,et al. Learning about Environmental Geometry: an Associative Model , 2022 .
[54] T. McNamara,et al. Where you are affects what you can easily imagine: Environmental geometry elicits sensorimotor interference in remote perspective taking , 2017, Cognition.
[55] J. Pearce,et al. A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .
[56] Matthew G. Buckley,et al. Crossing Boundaries: Global Reorientation Following Transfer From the Inside to the Outside of an Arena , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[57] A. Lew. Looking beyond the boundaries: time to put landmarks back on the cognitive map? , 2011, Psychological bulletin.
[58] E. Spelke,et al. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.
[59] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[60] Bradley R. Sturz,et al. Neither by global nor local cues alone: evidence for a unified orientation process , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[61] M. Cole,et al. Potentiation and overshadowing of shape by wall color in a kite-shaped maze using rats in a foraging task , 2011 .
[62] Craig K. Allison,et al. Overshadowing of geometric cues by a beacon in a spatial navigation task , 2013, Learning & behavior.
[63] Guillem R. Esber,et al. Reconciling the influence of predictiveness and uncertainty on stimulus salience: a model of attention in associative learning , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[64] Tobias Meilinger,et al. Putting Egocentric and Allocentric into Perspective , 2010, Spatial Cognition.
[65] C. Gallistel,et al. Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape , 1988 .
[66] J. Pearce,et al. The 36th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: An associative analysis of spatial learning , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[67] Debbie M. Kelly,et al. Pigeons' (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. , 1998 .
[68] Hasan Ayaz,et al. Maze Suite 1.0: A complete set of tools to prepare, present, and analyze navigational and spatial cognitive neuroscience experiments , 2008, Behavior research methods.
[69] Sara J Shettleworth,et al. An associative model of geometry learning: a modified choice rule. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[70] J. O’Keefe,et al. Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[71] E. Redhead,et al. Interaction between locale and taxon strategies in human spatial learning , 2007 .
[72] J. Pearce,et al. Between-cue associations influence searching for a hidden goal in an environment with a distinctive shape. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[73] M. Pelley. The Role of Associative History in Models of Associative Learning: A Selective Review and a Hybrid Model: , 2004 .
[74] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Using geometry to specify location: implications for spatial coding in children and nonhuman animals , 2007, Psychological research.
[75] Neil Burgess,et al. A neural-level model of spatial memory and imagery , 2018, eLife.
[76] M Zanforlin,et al. Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.
[77] J. O’Keefe,et al. Space in the brain: how the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[78] Paul N Wilson,et al. Enclosure shape influences cue competition effects and goal location learning , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[79] M. L. Le Pelley,et al. Overt attention and predictiveness in human contingency learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[80] J. Pearce,et al. Spatial Learning Based on Boundaries in Rats Is Hippocampus-Dependent and Prone to Overshadowing , 2010, Behavioral neuroscience.
[81] Pete R. Jones,et al. Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation , 2008, Current Biology.
[82] L. Hermer-Vazquez,et al. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks , 2001, Cognition.
[83] J. Rieser,et al. Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.
[84] Charlotte N. Boccara,et al. Grid cells in pre- and parasubiculum , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[85] Daniel Kernell,et al. Long-term plasticity , 2006 .
[86] M. Schachner,et al. Adult but not aged C57BL/6 male mice are capable of using geometry for orientation. , 2006, Learning & memory.
[87] T. Hartley,et al. The Neurobiology of Mammalian Navigation , 2018, Current Biology.
[88] Juan Pedro Vargas,et al. Encoding of geometric and featural spatial information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.
[89] Neil Burgess,et al. Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[90] P. Wilson,et al. Blocking of spatial learning between enclosure geometry and a local landmark. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[91] J Ward-Robinson,et al. Influence of a beacon on spatial learning based on the shape of the test environment. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[92] L. Nadel,et al. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .
[93] Mark Haselgrove,et al. Thinking outside of the box: Transfer of shape-based reorientation across the boundary of an arena , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.
[94] K. Jeffery,et al. The Boundary Vector Cell Model of Place Cell Firing and Spatial Memory , 2006, Reviews in the neurosciences.
[95] M. A. MacIver,et al. Neuroscience Needs Behavior: Correcting a Reductionist Bias , 2017, Neuron.
[96] Guillem R. Esber,et al. Absence of overshadowing between a landmark and geometric cues in a distinctively shaped environment: a test of Miller and Shettleworth (2007). , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[97] K. Jeffery,et al. Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Theoretical Accounts of Spatial Learning: a Neurobiological View(commentary on Pearce, 2009) , 2022 .
[98] J. Pearce,et al. Potentiation and overshadowing between landmarks and environmental geometric cues , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[99] J. Pearce,et al. Spatial learning based on the shape of the environment is influenced by properties of the objects forming the shape. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[100] Luca Tommasi,et al. Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) , 2004, Animal Cognition.
[101] Kent D. Bodily,et al. Is surface-based orientation influenced by a proportional relationship of shape parameters? , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.