Gaze-eccentricity effects on road position and steering.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Astros Chatziastros | Wilson O Readinger | Douglas W Cunningham | Heinrich H Bülthoff | James E Cutting | H. Bülthoff | J. Cutting | D. Cunningham | W. Readinger | A. Chatziastros
[1] Constance S. Royden,et al. From vision to action: experiments and models of steering control during driving. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] R. J. Bootsma,et al. Studies in Perception and Action III , 1995 .
[3] James A. Crowell,et al. Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements , 1996, Vision Research.
[4] J. Thomson,et al. Studies in perception and action V , 1999 .
[5] Takeo Watanabe,et al. High-Level Motion Processing , 1998 .
[6] E. S. Calvert,et al. Visual Judgments in Motion , 1954, Journal of Navigation.
[7] P. Atchley,et al. The effect of age, retinal eccentricity, and speed on the detection of optic flow components. , 1998, Psychology and aging.
[8] George H. Morris,et al. Hunter Seat Equitation , 1971 .
[9] J. Wann,et al. Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[10] Jacob Cohen. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[11] Paul A. Braren,et al. Wayfinding on foot from information in retinal, not optical, flow. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[12] Julie M. Harris,et al. Steering, Optic Flow, and the Respective Importance of Depth and Retinal Motion Distribution , 1999, Perception.
[13] A. V. van den Berg,et al. Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading , 1994, Nature.
[14] D. A. Owens,et al. Effects of luminance, blur, and age on nighttime visual guidance: A test of the selective degradation hypothesis , 1999 .
[15] Eli Brenner,et al. Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.
[16] M. Banks,et al. Perceiving heading with different retinal regions and types of optic flow , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[17] A. V. D. Berg,et al. Robustness of perception of heading from optic flow , 1992, Vision Research.
[18] M. Goodale,et al. The objects of action and perception , 1998, Cognition.
[19] W. H. Warren. The state of flow , 1998 .
[20] R. Wang,et al. Walking, looking to the side, and taking curved paths , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[21] A. Patla,et al. “Look where you’re going!”: gaze behaviour associated with maintaining and changing the direction of locomotion , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] Michael T. Turvey,et al. Wayfinding and the sampling of optical flow by eye movements. , 1996 .
[23] Vasa Rl. A study of visual performance. , 1960 .
[24] Julie M. Harris,et al. Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.
[25] M J Doherty,et al. People Don'T Keep Their Heads Still When Looking to One Side, and Other People Can Tell , 2001, Perception.
[26] Carel C. Koch,et al. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OPTOMETRY , 1939 .
[27] Li Li,et al. Perception of heading during rotation: sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects , 2000, Vision Research.
[28] G. Keppel. Design and analysis: A researcher's handbook, 3rd ed. , 1991 .
[29] J E Cutting,et al. Heading and path information from retinal flow in naturalistic environments , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[31] Richard A. Andersen,et al. Visual self-motion perception during head turns , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al. Drivers steer in their direction of gaze , 2004 .
[33] M Helander,et al. Drivers' Steering Behavior during Traffic Events: A Case of Perceptual Tropism? , 1978, Human factors.
[34] Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al. Seeking one’s heading through eye movements , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[35] David L. Strayer,et al. Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.
[36] D Regan,et al. How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.
[37] William H. Warren,et al. Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[38] R L VASA. A study of visual performance. , 1960, American journal of optometry and archives of American Academy of Optometry.
[39] HighWire Press. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.
[40] H Heuer,et al. Eccentric head positions bias random generation of leftward and rightward handle-bar rotations. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[41] P. Lachenbruch. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .
[42] Jens Rasmussen,et al. Skills, rules, and knowledge; signals, signs, and symbols, and other distinctions in human performance models , 1983, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.
[43] Daniel J. Hannon,et al. Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.
[44] J. Baird,et al. The locus of environmental attention , 1981 .
[45] D P Munoz,et al. Human eye-head gaze shifts in a distractor task. II. Reduced threshold for initiation of early head movements. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[46] David N. Lee,et al. Where we look when we steer , 1994, Nature.
[47] Thomas J Triggs. The Effect of Approaching Vehicles on the Lateral Position of Cars Travelling on a Two‐lane Rural Road , 1997 .
[48] Michael Land,et al. Which parts of the road guide steering? , 1995, Nature.
[49] Yoshiaki Shirai,et al. Look where you're going [robotic wheelchair] , 2003, IEEE Robotics Autom. Mag..
[50] M F Land,et al. The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.