Observing motor learning produces somatosensory change.

Observing the actions of others has been shown to affect motor learning, but does it have effects on sensory systems as well? It has been recently shown that motor learning that involves actual physical practice is also associated with plasticity in the somatosensory system. Here, we assessed the idea that observational learning likewise changes somatosensory function. We evaluated changes in somatosensory function after human subjects watched videos depicting motor learning. Subjects first observed video recordings of reaching movements either in a clockwise or counterclockwise force field. They were then trained in an actual force-field task that involved a counterclockwise load. Measures of somatosensory function were obtained before and after visual observation and also following force-field learning. Consistent with previous reports, video observation promoted motor learning. We also found that somatosensory function was altered following observational learning, both in direction and in magnitude, in a manner similar to that which occurs when motor learning is achieved through actual physical practice. Observation of the same sequence of movements in a randomized order did not result in somatosensory perceptual change. Observational learning and real physical practice appear to tap into the same capacity for sensory change in that subjects that showed a greater change following observational learning showed a reliably smaller change following physical motor learning. We conclude that effects of observing motor learning extend beyond the boundaries of traditional motor circuits, to include somatosensory representations.

[1]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  G. Luppino,et al.  Cortical connections of the inferior parietal cortical convexity of the macaque monkey. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  M. M. Taylor,et al.  Erratum and Note: PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 41, 782–787 (1967)] , 1967 .

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  R. Malach,et al.  Intersubject Synchronization of Cortical Activity During Natural Vision , 2004, Science.

[6]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[7]  C. Keysers,et al.  The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  D. Perrett,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.11 November 2004 Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective , 2022 .

[9]  Paul L. Gribble,et al.  Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation to the Primary Motor Cortex Interferes with Motor Learning by Observing , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  Spatially selective enhancement of proprioceptive acuity following motor learning. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[12]  Luca Turella,et al.  Neurofunctional modulation of brain regions by the observation of pointing and grasping actions. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  T. Milner,et al.  Functionally Specific Changes in Resting-State Sensorimotor Networks after Motor Learning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.

[16]  Sethu Vijayakumar,et al.  Unifying the Sensory and Motor Components of Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2008, NIPS.

[17]  R. Hari,et al.  Modulated Activation of the Human SI and SII Cortices during Observation of Hand Actions , 2002, NeuroImage.

[18]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  Davis M. Glasser,et al.  Effects of Action Observation on Physical Training After Stroke , 2008, Stroke.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Formation of a Motor Memory by Action Observation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  W. Cooper,et al.  Adaptation of phonetic feature analyzers for place of articulation. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  D. Henriques,et al.  Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Liana E Brown,et al.  Effect of trial order and error magnitude on motor learning by observing. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust , 2003, Neuron.

[25]  J. Kaas,et al.  Corticocortical connections of area 2 of somatosensory cortex in macaque monkeys: A correlative anatomical and electrophysiological study , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[26]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Somatosensation in social perception , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  M. M. Taylor,et al.  PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions , 1967 .

[29]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  Somatosensory Plasticity and Motor Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.