Early Cortical Distinction between Memories that Pertain to Ongoing Reality and Memories that Don't

Patients with anterior limbic lesions, in particular of the posterior orbitofrontal cortex, often act on the basis of memories that do not relate to ongoing reality and justify their behavior with invented stories that can mostly be traced back to real events (spontaneous confabulation). Recent studies demonstrated that the patients fail to suppress activated memory traces that do not pertain to ongoing reality. In the present study, we used a similar paradigm and high-resolution event-related potentials to explore when this suppression happens. Healthy subjects made two runs of a continuous recognition task, composed of the same set of pictures, and were requested to indicate picture recurrences only within the ongoing run. Thus, performance in the first run depends on new learning, whereas the second run requires the ability to realize whether a picture is solely familiar from its occurrence in the previous run ('distracter') or whether it has already appeared in the ongoing second run ('target'). We find that correct rejection (suppression) of currently irrelevant pictures (distracters of run 2) is associated with absent negative deflection of a frontal potential and absence of a specific cortical potential map configuration after 220-300 ms. By contrast, learning and recognition of repeatedly presented information is associated with cortical amplitude modulation after 400-480 ms. These findings indicate that by the time the content of a mental association is recognized and consolidated, its cortical representation has already been adjusted according to whether it relates to ongoing reality or not. This sequence may also explain the ability to distinguish between the memory of a true event and the memory of a thought.

[1]  Valerie Treyer,et al.  Selection of Currently Relevant Memories by the Human Posterior Medial Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  R. Ptak,et al.  Hypothalamic amnesia with spontaneous confabulations: A clinicopathologic study , 2001, Neurology.

[3]  W. Schultz,et al.  Neuronal activity in monkey ventral striatum related to the expectation of reward , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  G. Percheron,et al.  [Informational analysis of the basal ganglia related system]. , 1994, Revue neurologique.

[5]  R. Henson,et al.  Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  J. DeLuca,et al.  Confabulation Following Aneurysm of the Anterior Communicating Artery , 1991, Cortex.

[7]  Alan J. Parkin,et al.  Impaired Temporal Context Memory on Anterograde But Not Retrograde Tests in the Absence of Frontal Pathology , 1993, Cortex.

[8]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[9]  D Friedman,et al.  Cognitive event-related potential components during continuous recognition memory for pictures. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[10]  P W Burgess,et al.  Confabulation and the control of recollection. , 1996, Memory.

[11]  E. Rolls The Brain and Emotion , 2000 .

[12]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[13]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Electrophysiological brain activity and memory source monitoring. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[14]  J. Fuster Memory in the cerebral cortex , 1994 .

[15]  Charles M. Butter,et al.  Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta , 1969 .

[16]  A. Schnider,et al.  The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  E. Lynd-Balta,et al.  The orbital and medial prefrontal circuit through the primate basal ganglia , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Principles of spatial analysis , 1987 .

[19]  L. V. D. Horst Über die Psychologie des Korsakowsyndroms. , 1932 .

[20]  A. Schnider,et al.  Disorientation in amnesia. A confusion of memory traces. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Neuroanatomical Correlates of Veridical and Illusory Recognition Memory: Evidence from Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, Neuron.

[22]  R. Ptak,et al.  Spontaneous confabulations after Orbitofrontal damage: The role of temporal context confusion and self-monitoring , 1999 .

[23]  C M Michel,et al.  42-channel potential map series to visual contrast and stereo stimuli: perceptual and cognitive event-related segments. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[24]  D. Schacter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[25]  C W Hess,et al.  Memory without context: amnesia with confabulations after infarction of the right capsular genu. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[26]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  T Landis,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for fast visual processing through the human koniocellular pathway when stimuli move. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  False memories and confabulation , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  A. Damasio Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[30]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Human Cognition. , 1999, News in physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society.

[31]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  New insights into the Stroop effect : a spatiotemporal analysis of electric brain activity , 2000 .

[32]  P. Viviani,et al.  Internally driven vs. externally cued movement selection: a study on the timing of brain activity. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  C. Michel,et al.  Unraveling the cerebral dynamics of mental imagery , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[34]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980 .

[35]  W Singer,et al.  Consciousness and the structure of neuronal representations. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[36]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  The Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Recognition Memory and Memory for Source: An fMRI Study , 1999, NeuroImage.

[37]  G. Barba,et al.  Confabulation: Knowledge and recollective experience , 1993 .

[38]  Raymond P. Kesner,et al.  Item and order dissociation in humans with prefrontal cortex damage , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  R. Ptak,et al.  Recovery from spontaneous confabulations parallels recovery of temporal confusion in memory , 2000, Neurology.

[41]  M. Kopelman,et al.  Confabulation Extending across Episodic, Personal, and General Semantic Memory , 1997 .

[42]  Gabriel Leonard,et al.  Frontal-lobe contribution to recency judgements , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Joaquin M. Fuster,et al.  Single cell activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys , 1981, Brain Research.

[44]  E Zarahn,et al.  Activation of the prefrontal cortex during judgments of recency: a functional MRI study. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[45]  Felicia A. Huppert,et al.  Recognition Memory in Amnesic Patients: Effect of Temporal Context and Familiarity of Material , 1976, Cortex.

[46]  N. Stanhope,et al.  Temporal and spatial context memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic lesions , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Mieke Verfaellie,et al.  Impaired acquisition of temporal information in retrosplenial amnesia , 1988, Brain and Cognition.

[48]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Confabulation, Memory Deficits, and Frontal Dysfunction , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[49]  Paul W. Burgess,et al.  Content-Specific Confabulation , 1999, Cortex.

[50]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  FALSE RECALL AND FALSE RECOGNITION: AN EXAMINATION OF THE EFFECTS OF SELECTIVE AND COMBINED LESIONS TO THE MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE/DIENCEPHALON AND FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES , 1999 .

[51]  S. Thorpe,et al.  The orbitofrontal cortex: Neuronal activity in the behaving monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  Armin Schnider,et al.  Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  Lars Nyberg,et al.  Brain Regions Differentially Involved in Remembering What and When: a PET Study , 1997, Neuron.

[55]  David Friedman,et al.  ERPs during continuous recognition memory for words , 1990, Biological Psychology.

[56]  T Landis,et al.  Spatio-temporal analysis of electric brain activity during semantic and phonological word processing. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[57]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Memory with and without awareness: performance and electrophysiological evidence of savings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[58]  A. Dale,et al.  Late Onset of Anterior Prefrontal Activity during True and False Recognition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[59]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions , 1989, Neuropsychologia.