The Effects of Emotional Arousal and Valence on Television Viewers' Cognitive Capacity and Memory.

This study examines the combined effects of arousal and valence on viewers’ capacity allocation to and memory for television messages. Results show that when valence (how positive or negative a message is) is controlled, arousing messages are remembered better than calm messages. When arousal is controlled, positive messages are remembered better than negative messages. Reaction time results suggest that capacity allocation is a function of both valence and arousal. Viewers allocate the most capacity to positive arousing messages and the least capacity to negative arousing messages. The calm messages (both positive and negative) fall between these two.

[1]  D. Hebb Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). , 1955, Psychological review.

[2]  C. Osgood,et al.  The Measurement of Meaning , 1958 .

[3]  Elizabeth Duffy,et al.  Activation and behavior , 1962 .

[4]  D. Zillmann Excitation transfer in communication-mediated aggressive behavior , 1971 .

[5]  D. Zillmann,et al.  Motivated aggressiveness perpetuated by exposure to aggressive films and reduced by exposure to nonaggressive films , 1973 .

[6]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[7]  James H. Watt,et al.  An Information Theory Measure for Television Programming , 1974 .

[8]  Elizabeth Pugzles Lorch,et al.  The effects of TV program pacing on the behavior of preschool children , 1977 .

[9]  J. Russell,et al.  Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions , 1977 .

[10]  J. Watt,et al.  AN EXAMINATION OF THREE MODELS OF TELEVISION VIEWING AND AGGRESSION , 1977 .

[11]  Peter J. Lang,et al.  A Bio‐Informational Theory of Emotional Imagery , 1979 .

[12]  D. Pearl,et al.  Television and behavior : ten years of scientific progress and implications for the eighties , 1982 .

[13]  Alexis S. Tan Mass communication theories and research , 1985 .

[14]  Esther Thorson,et al.  Attention to Local and Global Complexity in Television Messages , 1987 .

[15]  P. Lang,et al.  Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. , 1989 .

[16]  B. Reeves,et al.  Emotional Television Scenes and Hemispheric Specialization. , 1989 .

[17]  Jeanne M. Meadowcroft,et al.  Influence of Story Schema Development on Children's Attention to Television , 1989 .

[18]  C. Atkin,et al.  Effects of negative political advertising on the political process , 1990 .

[19]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. , 1990, Psychological review.

[20]  M. Basil,et al.  Negative and Positive Television Messages , 1991 .

[21]  John E. Newhagen,et al.  The Evening's Bad News: Effects of Compelling Negative Television News Images on Memory , 1992 .

[22]  E. Thorson,et al.  The Effects of Television Videographics and Lecture Familiarity on Adult Cardiac Orienting Responses and Memory , 1992 .

[23]  M. Bradley,et al.  Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  M. Shapiro,et al.  Comparing Positive and Negative Political Advertising on Radio , 1992 .

[25]  Seth Geiger,et al.  The Effects of Scene Changes and Semantic Relatedness on Attention to Television , 1993 .

[26]  Annie Lang,et al.  Emotion, Hemispheric Specialization, and Visual and Verbal Memory for Television Messages , 1993 .

[27]  Annie Lang,et al.  The Effects of Related and Unrelated Cuts on Television Viewers' Attention, Processing Capacity, and Memory , 1993 .

[28]  M. Bradley,et al.  Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[29]  Annie Lang,et al.  Tell me a story: Narrative elaboration and memory for television , 1995 .

[30]  P. Lang The Cognitive Psychophysiology of Emotion , 2019, Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders.