The Native Mind : Biological Categorization , Reasoning , and Decision Making in Development and Across Cultures

This paper describes a cross-cultural and developmental research project on naïve or folk biology – that is, the study of how people conceptualize nature. Our methods and results integrate three distinct perspectives into a complex and coherent account of biological cognition. From the standpoint of mainstream cognitive psychology, we find that results gathered from “standard populations” in industrialized societies often fail to generalize to humanity at large. For example, similarity-driven typicality and diversity effects and basic level phenomena either are not found or pattern differently when we move beyond undergraduate participants. A second perspective involves the notion that domain-specific modules facilitate and structure cognition. Here again, standard populations may yield misleading results because such populations represent examples of especially impoverished experience with respect to nature. Conceptions of humans as biological kinds vary with cultural milieu and input conditions. We also show certain phenomena that are robust across populations, consistent with notions of domain-specificity. Third, we argue that cultural transmission and formation does not consist primarily in shared rules or norms, but in complex distributions of causally-connected representations across minds. This has novel implications for environmental decision making and management, including dealings with commons problems. Our framework addresses a series of methodological issues, such as the fallacy of conceiving culture to be a bounded entity, well-defined system, or independent variable, and the tendency to “essentialize” culture and treat it as an explanation rather than phenomena to be explained.

[1]  D. E. Breedlove,et al.  General Principles of Classification and Nomenclature in Folk Biology , 1973 .

[2]  D. Medin,et al.  Cultural and experiential differences in the development of folkbiological induction , 2003 .

[3]  C. Lumsden Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London (1985), viii, +301. Price $29.95 , 1986 .

[4]  D. Barash The Biology of Moral Systems, Richard D. Alexander. Aldine, Hawthorne, New York (1987), xx, +301. Price $34.95 hardback, $16.95 paperback , 1987 .

[5]  D. Medin,et al.  Does rank have its privilege? Inductive inferences within folkbiological taxonomies , 1997, Cognition.

[6]  W. Durham,et al.  Colonization, development, and deforestation in Petén, northern Guatemala. , 1995 .

[7]  James S. Boster,et al.  Exchange of Varieties and Information Between Aguaruna Manioc Cultivators , 1986 .

[8]  Terry Kit-fong Au,et al.  Mechanical causality in children's 'folkbiology' , 1999 .

[9]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The development of features in object concepts , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  Cecil H. Brown,et al.  some general principles of biological and non‐biological folk classification1 , 1976 .

[11]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The evolution of cooperation. , 1984, Science.

[12]  D. Medin,et al.  Tall is typical: Central tendency, ideal dimensions, and graded category structure among tree experts and novices , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[13]  D. Medin,et al.  Folkecology and commons management in the Maya Lowlands. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Running Head: CULTURE AND THOUGHT Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic vs. Analytic Cognition , 2001 .

[15]  James S. Boster,et al.  The information economy model applied to biological similarity judgment , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[16]  P. Harris,et al.  Belief‐Desire Reasoning among Baka Children: Evidence for a Universal Conception of Mind , 1991 .

[17]  Heather M. Coon,et al.  Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  J. D. Dougherty LEARNING NAMES FOR PLANTS AND PLANTS FOR NAMES , 1979 .

[19]  S. Atran Classifying nature across cultures , 1995 .

[20]  M. Strevens The essentialist aspect of naive theories , 2000, Cognition.

[21]  James S. Boster,et al.  Natural and Human Sources of Cross‐Cultural Agreement in Ornithological Classification , 1989 .

[22]  Niles Eldredge,et al.  Information, Economics, and Evolution , 1986 .

[23]  Brian Stross,et al.  VI. ACQUISITION OF BOTANICAL TERMINOLOGY BY TZELTAL CHILDREN , 1973 .

[24]  Arlen F. Chase,et al.  Itza Maya Tropical Agro-Forestry [and Comments and Replies] , 1993, Current Anthropology.

[25]  Rita Astuti “the Vezo are not a kind of people”: identity, difference, and “ethnicity” among a fishing people of western Madagascar , 1995 .

[26]  Keith S. Donnellan Necessity and Criteria , 1962 .

[27]  D. Cohen Cultural variation: considerations and implications. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  C. Kalish Preschoolers' Understanding of Germs as Causes of Illness. , 1993 .

[29]  G. Hardin,et al.  The Tragedy of the Commons , 1968, Green Planet Blues.

[30]  R. Lewontin ‘The Selfish Gene’ , 1977, Nature.

[31]  N. Bird-David,et al.  “Animism” Revisited , 1999, Current Anthropology.

[32]  Edward O. Wilson,et al.  Genes, Mind and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. , 1982 .

[33]  D L Medin,et al.  Expertise and category-based induction. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  S. Atran The Nature of Folk‐Botanical Life Forms , 1985 .

[35]  L. Cosmides,et al.  The Adapted mind : evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture , 1992 .

[36]  D L Medin,et al.  Concepts and conceptual structure. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[37]  Frank C. Keil,et al.  The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. , 1995 .

[38]  P. Richerson,et al.  Norms and Bounded Rationality , 2022 .

[39]  E. Ostrom A Behavioral Approach to the Rational Choice Theory of Collective Action: Presidential Address, American Political Science Association, 1997 , 1998, American Political Science Review.

[40]  E. Rosch The nature of mental codes for color categories. , 1975 .

[41]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  Deriving Categories to Achieve Goals , 1991 .

[42]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[43]  S. Sloman Feature-Based Induction , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  D. Medin,et al.  Inductive reasoning in folkbiological thought. , 1999 .

[45]  L. Rips,et al.  Necessity and natural categories. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[46]  S. Atran Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  D. Sperber,et al.  Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach , 1998 .

[48]  S. Carey,et al.  Like father, like son: young children's understanding of how and why offspring resemble their parents. , 1996, Child development.

[49]  Scott Atran,et al.  Ordinary constraints on the semantics of living kinds , 1987 .

[50]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[51]  D. Feeny,et al.  The benefits of the commons , 1989, Nature.

[52]  L. Barsalou Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[54]  Susan Carey,et al.  On the origin of causal understanding. , 1995 .

[55]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  God's beliefs versus mother's: the development of nonhuman agent concepts. , 2001, Child development.

[56]  J. Boster,et al.  Form or Function: A Comparison of Expert and Novice Judgments of Similarity Among Fish , 1989 .

[57]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Tree of Life: Universal and Cultural Features of Folkbiological Taxonomies and Inductions , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  S. Boysen,et al.  Spontaneous discrimination of natural stimuli by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[59]  R. Bulmer Folk biology in the New Guinea Highlands , 1974 .

[60]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[61]  T. Tyler,et al.  Power Asymmetries and the Ethical Atmosphere in Negotiations , 2001 .

[62]  Jean-Baptiste Van der Henst,et al.  When is a conclusion worth deriving? A relevance-based analysis of indeterminate relational problems , 2002 .

[63]  D. Medin,et al.  Categorization and Reasoning among Tree Experts: Do All Roads Lead to Rome? , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[64]  S. Atran Hamula organisation and Masha'a tenure in Palestine , 1986 .

[65]  J. Coley,et al.  Generic species and basic levels: Essence and appearance in folk biology , 1997 .

[66]  D. Dennett Darwin's Dangerous Idea , 1995 .

[67]  James S. Boster,et al.  Developmental Changes within the Structure of the Mammal Domain. , 1992 .

[68]  Patrick Shafto,et al.  Why essences are essential in the psychology of concepts , 2001, Cognition.

[69]  David A. Zubin,et al.  Gender and Folk Taxonomy , 1986 .

[70]  Dan Sperber,et al.  In defense of massive modularity , 2001 .

[71]  Phillip Wolff,et al.  Evolution and devolution of folkbiological knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[72]  Dan Sperber,et al.  Learning without teaching: Its place in culture. , 1991 .

[73]  R. Rappaport,et al.  Ecology, Meaning, and Religion , 1979 .

[74]  D. Medin,et al.  The semantic side of decision making , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[75]  B. Malinowski Argonauts of the Western Pacific , 1922 .

[76]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[77]  F. Gil-White,et al.  Are Ethnic Groups Biological “Species” to the Human Brain? , 2001, Current Anthropology.

[78]  Elinor Ostrom,et al.  Governing the commons , 1990 .

[79]  Ousa,et al.  Folkbiology doesn ’ t Come from Folkpsychology : Evidence from Yukatek Maya in Cross-Cultural Perspective , 2001 .

[80]  R. D'Andrade,et al.  Why a successful concept should not be discarded. Commentaries. Author's reply , 1999 .

[81]  W. Batchelder,et al.  Culture as Consensus: A Theory of Culture and Informant Accuracy , 1986 .

[82]  Cecil H. Brown Language and living things: Uniformities in folk classification and naming , 1984 .

[83]  Kayoko Inagaki,et al.  The effects of raising animals on children's biological knowledge , 1990 .

[84]  G. D. Fuller,et al.  The Vegetation of Peten , 1937 .

[85]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. , 1999 .

[86]  D. Sperber The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. , 1994 .

[87]  Dennis E. Breedlove,et al.  Principles of Tzeltal Plant Classification , 1975 .

[88]  E. M. Evans,et al.  Cognitive and Contextual Factors in the Emergence of Diverse Belief Systems: Creation versus Evolution , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[89]  D. Medin,et al.  Comments on part I: psychological essentialism , 1989 .

[90]  J. Mandler,et al.  Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[91]  S. Carey,et al.  Zafimaniry: An Understanding of What Is Passed on from Parents to Children: A Cross-Cultural Investigation , 2001 .

[92]  L. Rips Inductive judgments about natural categories. , 1975 .

[93]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[94]  J. Tanaka,et al.  Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[95]  P. Schyns,et al.  Categorization creates functional features , 1997 .

[96]  D. Medin,et al.  A relevance theory of induction , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[97]  S. Atran The Case for Modularity: Sin or Salvation? , 2001 .

[98]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Category-Based Induction , 1990 .

[99]  D. Sperber,et al.  Anthropology and Psychology: Towards an Epidemiology of Representations , 1985 .