Event-related near-infrared spectroscopy detects conflict in the motor cortex in a Stroop task

The Stroop effect is one of the most popular models of conflict processing in neuroscience and psychology. The response conflict theory of the Stroop effect explains decreased performance in the incongruent condition of Stroop tasks by assuming that the task-relevant and the task-irrelevant stimulus features elicit conflicting response tendencies. However, to date, there is not much explicit neural evidence supporting this theory. Here we used functional near-infrared imaging (fNIRS) to examine whether conflict at the level of the motor cortex can be detected in the incongruent relative to the congruent condition of a Stroop task. Response conflict was determined by comparing the activity of the hemisphere ipsilateral to the response hand in the congruent and incongruent conditions. First, results provided explicit hemodynamic evidence supporting the response conflict theory of the Stroop effect: there was greater motor cortex activation in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the response hand in the incongruent than in the congruent condition during the initial stage of the hemodynamic response. Second, as fNIRS is still a relatively novel technology, it is methodologically significant that our data shows that fNIRS is able to detect a brief and transient increase in hemodynamic activity localized to the motor cortex, which in this study is related to subthreshold motor response activation.

[1]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development—an event-related fNIRS study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[2]  L. Mulder,et al.  Use of partial stimulus information in response processing. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Wim Vanduffel,et al.  Fast optical signal not detected in awake behaving monkeys , 2008 .

[4]  C. Eriksen,et al.  An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .

[5]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Stimulus and response conflict in the color–word Stroop task: A combined electro-myography and event-related potential study , 2010, Brain Research.

[6]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[7]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping , 2004, NeuroImage.

[8]  Patrik Pluchino,et al.  A hemodynamic correlate of lateralized visual short-term memories , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  D. Delpy,et al.  Measurement of Cranial Optical Path Length as a Function of Age Using Phase Resolved Near Infrared Spectroscopy , 1994 .

[10]  E. Gratton,et al.  On-line optical imaging of the human brain with 160-ms temporal resolution. , 2000, Optics express.

[11]  Dénes Szücs,et al.  The development of inhibitory control: An averaged and single-trial Lateralized Readiness Potential study , 2011, NeuroImage.

[12]  B. Burle,et al.  Sequential compatibility effects and cognitive control: does conflict really matter? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  B. Chance,et al.  Cognition-activated low-frequency modulation of light absorption in human brain. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[16]  Luciano Gamberini,et al.  Selective activation of the superior frontal gyrus in task-switching: An event-related fNIRS study , 2008, NeuroImage.

[17]  Dénes Szücs,et al.  Real-time Tracking of Motor Response Activation and Response Competition in a Stroop Task in Young Children: A Lateralized Readiness Potential Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Mayhew,et al.  Fine detail of neurovascular coupling revealed by spatiotemporal analysis of the hemodynamic response to single whisker stimulation in rat barrel cortex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy detects specific inferior-frontal activation during incongruent Stroop trials , 2005, Biological Psychology.

[20]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[21]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Near-infrared optical topography to assess activation of the parietal cortex during a visuo-spatial task , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  A new method based on ICBM152 head surface for probe placement in multichannel fNIRS , 2011, NeuroImage.

[23]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[24]  John Morton,et al.  Selective Attention to Words and Colours , 1973 .

[25]  Dénes Szücs,et al.  Motor conflict in Stroop tasks: Direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography , 2009, NeuroImage.

[26]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number/size congruency paradigm: An ERP study , 2008, Brain Research.

[27]  C W Eriksen,et al.  Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Ying Xiong,et al.  The Triphasic Intrinsic Signal: Implications for Functional Imaging , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  E. Gratton,et al.  A continuously variable frequency cross-correlation phase fluorometer with picosecond resolution. , 1983, Biophysical journal.

[30]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[31]  V. Csépe,et al.  The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[32]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[33]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The Brain Locus of Interaction between Number and Size: A Combined Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Event-related Potential Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Yevgeniy B. Sirotin,et al.  Spatiotemporal precision and hemodynamic mechanism of optical point spreads in alert primates , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Hellmuth Obrig,et al.  The fast optical signal—Robust or elusive when non-invasively measured in the human adult? , 2005, NeuroImage.

[36]  David A. Boas,et al.  Evidence that cerebral blood volume can provide brain activation maps with better spatial resolution than deoxygenated hemoglobin , 2005, NeuroImage.

[37]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[38]  A. Villringer,et al.  Near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): A new tool to study hemodynamic changes during activation of brain function in human adults , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  An event-related functional MRI study of the stroop color word interference task. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  Yevgeniy B. Sirotin,et al.  Reply to Uludağ: fMRI “initial dip” reflects increase in oxygenated hemoglobin , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Bülent Sankur,et al.  Multilevel Statistical Inference From Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Data During Stroop Interference , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[43]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[44]  G. Lohmann,et al.  Color-Word Matching Stroop Task: Separating Interference and Response Conflict , 2001, NeuroImage.

[45]  Frithjof Kruggel,et al.  Near‐infrared spectroscopy can detect brain activity during a color–word matching Stroop task in an event‐related design , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[46]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[47]  D. Delpy,et al.  System for long-term measurement of cerebral blood and tissue oxygenation on newborn infants by near infra-red transillumination , 1988, Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing.

[48]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  J. Leon-Carrion,et al.  The hemodynamics of cognitive control: The level of concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin in the superior prefrontal cortex varies as a function of performance in a modified Stroop task , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[50]  Frithjof Kruggel,et al.  Age dependency of the hemodynamic response as measured by functional near-infrared spectroscopy , 2003, NeuroImage.

[51]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[52]  Tobias Egner,et al.  The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[53]  S. Ogawa,et al.  BOLD Based Functional MRI at 4 Tesla Includes a Capillary Bed Contribution: Echo‐Planar Imaging Correlates with Previous Optical Imaging Using Intrinsic Signals , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[54]  Ludovico Minati,et al.  Emotional modulation of visual cortex activity: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2009, Neuroreport.

[55]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[57]  Monica Fabiani,et al.  When in Doubt, Do it Both Ways: Brain Evidence of the Simultaneous Activation of Conflicting Motor Responses in a Spatial Stroop Task , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  O. Gruber,et al.  Decomposing interference during Stroop performance into different conflict factors: An event-related fMRI study , 2009, Cortex.

[59]  Simone Cutini,et al.  Functional near Infrared Optical Imaging in Cognitive Neuroscience: An Introductory Review , 2012 .