Combined Space and Alertness Related Therapy of Visual Hemineglect: Effect of Therapy Frequency

The combined efficacy of space- and alertness related training in chronic hemineglect was tested behaviorally and in a longitudinal fMRI study. Earlier results had shown that both space as well as alertness related training as single intervention methods lead to short term improvement which, however, is not stable for longer time periods. The neurobiological data obtained in these studies revealed differential cortical reorganization patterns for the two training approaches thereby leading to the hypothesis that a combination of both trainings might result in stronger and longer lasting effects. The results of our current study, however, – at least at first glance – do not clearly corroborate this hypothesis, because neither alertness training alone nor the combination with OKS on the group level led to significant behavioral improvement, although four of the six patients after alertness and even more after combined training showed a higher percentage of behavioral improvement than during baseline. Despite the lack of clearcut behavioral training induced improvement we found right parietal or fronto-parietal increase of activation in the imaging data immediately after combined training and at follow-up 3 weeks later. The study design had called for splitting up training time between the two training approaches in order to match total training time with our earlier single training studies. The results of our current study are discussed as a possible consequence of reduced training time and intensity of both training measures under the combined training situation.

[1]  Mario Bonato,et al.  Hemispatial neglect : computer-based testing allows more sensitive quantification of attentional disorders and recovery and might lead to better evaluation of rehabilitation , 2013 .

[2]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. , 2013 .

[3]  M. Bonato,et al.  Neglect and Extinction Depend Greatly on Task Demands: A Review , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Spatial neglect and attention networks. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  W. Schupp,et al.  Auswirkungen eines Aufmerksamkeitstrainings auf die aphasische Symptomatik bei Schlaganfallpatienten , 2011 .

[6]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Increased attentional demands impair contralesional space awareness following stroke , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  N. Sadato,et al.  Neural substrates of phasic alertness: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2010, Neurosciences research.

[8]  Joseph M. DeGutis,et al.  Human Neuroscience , 2022 .

[9]  G. Fink,et al.  Recovery from hemineglect: Differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training , 2009, Cortex.

[10]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Left unilateral neglect as a disconnection syndrome. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  G. R. Fink,et al.  Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: A behavioural and fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  K. Willmes,et al.  Spatial attention: more than intrinsic alerting? , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  H. Duffau,et al.  Direct Evidence for a Parietal-Frontal Pathway Subserving Spatial Awareness in Humans , 2005, Science.

[14]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[15]  Karsten Specht,et al.  Functional reorganisation in patients with right hemisphere stroke after training of alertness: a longitudinal PET and fMRI study in eight cases , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Klaus Willmes,et al.  Network for auditory intrinsic alertness: a PET study , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  A. Cantagallo,et al.  Specific Computerized Attention Training in Stroke and Traumatic Brain-Injured Patients , 2003 .

[18]  R. Teasell,et al.  Intensity of Aphasia Therapy, Impact on Recovery , 2003, Stroke.

[19]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  F. Meneghello,et al.  Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  H. Hämäläinen,et al.  Experiences of combined visual training and arm activation in the rehabilitation of unilateral visual neglect: A clinical study , 2002 .

[22]  K. Willmes,et al.  On the Functional Neuroanatomy of Intrinsic and Phasic Alertness , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Do We Need the “Lateral” in Unilateral Neglect? Spatially Nonselective Attention Deficits in Unilateral Neglect and Their Implications for Rehabilitation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural consequences of competing stimuli in both visual hemifields: A physiological basis for visual extinction , 2000, Annals of neurology.

[25]  C Braun,et al.  Differential Activation in Somatosensory Cortex for Different Discrimination Tasks , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[27]  D. Nico Effectiveness of sensory stimulation on tactile extinction , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  I. Radermacher,et al.  Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidencefor a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in theright hemisphere , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  C. Marquardt,et al.  Visual background motion reduces size distortion in spatial neglect. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[30]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[31]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[32]  S. Huber,et al.  Computer assisted retraining of attentional impairments in patients with multiple sclerosis , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[33]  S Ekholm,et al.  Nonlateralized attentional deficits: an important component behind persisting visuospatial neglect? , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[34]  Tom Manly,et al.  Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  L. Pizzamiglio,et al.  Motor deficits and optokinetic stimulation in patients with left hemineglect , 1997, Neurology.

[36]  G. Vallar,et al.  Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Dissociation between Position Sense and Visual-spatial Components of Hemineglect through a Specific Rehabilitation Treatment , 2022 .

[37]  K. Willmes,et al.  Do Specific Attention Deficits Need Specific Training , 1997 .

[38]  Diego Fernandez-Duque,et al.  Relating the mechanisms of orienting and alerting , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Kerstin Tham,et al.  Sustained attention and awareness of disability in chronic neglect , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  R. Nudo,et al.  Neural Substrates for the Effects of Rehabilitative Training on Motor Recovery After Ischemic Infarct , 1996, Science.

[41]  H. Karnath,et al.  Optokinetic stimulation influences the disturbed perception of body orientation in spatial neglect. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[42]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  Optokinetic Stimulation Affects Both Vertical and Horizontal Deficits of Position Sense in Unilateral Neglect , 1995, Cortex.

[43]  I. Nimmo-Smith,et al.  Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitation implications. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[44]  S. Paolucci,et al.  Effectiveness of neglect rehabilitation in a randomized group study. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[45]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Recovery from directional hypokinesia and bradykinesia in unilateral neglect. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[46]  H. Karnath,et al.  Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  H. Karnath Disturbed coordinate transformation in the neural representation of space as the crucial mechanism leading to neglect , 1994 .

[48]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  Deficits of position sense, unilateral neglect and optokinetic stimulation , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  H. Karnath,et al.  Decrease of contralateral neglect by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[51]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  F. Newcombe,et al.  Disorders of space exploration and cognition E. De Renzi. John Wiley, Chichester (1982). 268 pp. , 1982, Neuroscience.

[53]  K. Heilman,et al.  Right hemisphere dominance for attention , 1980, Neurology.

[54]  K. Heilman,et al.  Hypoarousal in patients with the neglect syndrome and emotional indifference , 1978, Neurology.

[55]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[56]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  A. Sack,et al.  Comparison of fMRI activation patterns for test and training procedures of alertness and focused attention. , 2013, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[58]  G. Fink,et al.  Alertness-training in neglect: behavioral and imaging results. , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[59]  C. Marquardt,et al.  Repetitive optokinetic stimulation induces lasting recovery from visual neglect. , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[60]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Intensivierung, Fokussierung und Verhaltensrelevanz , 2004 .

[61]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[62]  I. Robertson,et al.  The effect of contralesional limb activation training and sustained attention training for self-care programmes in unilateral spatial neglect. , 2000, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[63]  G. Kerkhoff Rehabilitation of Visuospatial Cognition and Visual Exploration in Neglect: a Cross-over Study. , 1998, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[64]  G. Vallar,et al.  Transcutaneous electrical stimulation of the neck muscles and hemineglect rehabilitation. , 1996, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[65]  I. Robertson The relationship between lateralised and non-lateralised attentional deficits in unilateral neglect. , 1993 .

[66]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Functional reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in adult owl monkeys after behaviorally controlled tactile stimulation. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[68]  K M Heilman,et al.  [Right hemisphere dominance for attention]. , 1983, Revue neurologique.