Expectations for tonal cadences: Sensory and cognitive priming effects

Studies examining the formation of melodic and harmonic expectations during music listening have repeatedly demonstrated that a tonal context primes listeners to expect certain (tonally related) continuations over others. However, few such studies have (1) selected stimuli using ready examples of expectancy violation derived from real-world instances of tonal music, (2) provided a consistent account for the influence of sensory and cognitive mechanisms on tonal expectancies by comparing different computational simulations, or (3) combined melodic and harmonic representations in modelling cognitive processes of expectation. To resolve these issues, this study measures expectations for the most recurrent cadence patterns associated with tonal music and then simulates the reported findings using three sensory–cognitive models of auditory expectation. In Experiment 1, participants provided explicit retrospective expectancy ratings both before and after hearing the target melodic tone and chord of the cadential formula. In Experiment 2, participants indicated as quickly as possible whether those target events were in or out of tune relative to the preceding context. Across both experiments, cadences terminating with stable melodic tones and chords elicited the highest expectancy ratings and the fastest and most accurate responses. Moreover, the model simulations supported a cognitive interpretation of tonal processing, in which listeners with exposure to tonal music generate expectations as a consequence of the frequent (co-)occurrence of events on the musical surface.

[1]  E. Margulis A Model of Melodic Expectation , 2005 .

[2]  R. Parncutt Harmony: A Psychoacoustical Approach , 1989 .

[3]  William A. Sethares,et al.  Consonance-Based Spectral Mappings , 1998 .

[4]  Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis,et al.  Timbre priming effects and expectation in melody , 2006 .

[5]  J. Wilkey Music, the Arts, and Ideas , 1969 .

[6]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Stephen McAdams,et al.  Simulating melodic and harmonic expectations for tonal cadences using probabilistic models , 2018 .

[8]  Jobst Peter Fricke,et al.  Kadenzwahrnehmung und Kadenzgeschichte : ein Beitrag zu einer Grammatik der Musik = Cadence perception and the history of the cadence : a contribution to a grammar of music , 1992 .

[9]  Janet Schmalfeldt,et al.  Cadential processes: The evaded cadence and the “one more time” technique , 1992 .

[10]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Robert O. Gjerdingen,et al.  A Classic Turn of Phrase: Music and the Psychology of Convention , 1988 .

[12]  Edward W. Large,et al.  A Neurodynamic Account of Musical Tonality , 2016 .

[13]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[14]  Barbara Tillmann,et al.  Musical expectations within chord sequences: Facilitation due to tonal stability without closure effects. , 2013 .

[15]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. , 1982 .

[16]  David R. W. Sears,et al.  THE PERCEPTION OF CADENTIAL CLOSURE , 2015 .

[17]  R. Plomp,et al.  Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  Probabilistic models of expectation violation predict psychophysiological emotional responses to live concert music , 2013, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[19]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-Organizing Maps , 2010 .

[20]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[21]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  Perceived harmonic structure of chords in three related musical keys. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Ian H. Witten,et al.  Data Compression Using Adaptive Coding and Partial String Matching , 1984, IEEE Trans. Commun..

[23]  David R. W. Sears,et al.  Perceiving the Classical Cadence , 2014 .

[24]  Brady T. West,et al.  Linear Mixed Models: A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software , 2006 .

[25]  S. Koelsch,et al.  Effects of Unexpected Chords and of Performer's Expression on Brain Responses and Electrodermal Activity , 2008, PloS one.

[26]  Burton S. Rosner,et al.  Harmonic Closure: Music Theory and Perception , 1992 .

[27]  M. Pearce,et al.  Tracking of pitch probabilities in congenital amusia , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  F N Martin,et al.  Reconsidering the limits of normal hearing. , 2000, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.

[29]  P. Janata,et al.  A combined model of sensory and cognitive representations underlying tonal expectations in music: from audio signals to behavior. , 2014, Psychological review.

[30]  E. Bigand,et al.  More About the Musical Expertise of Musically Untrained Listeners , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  C. Stevens,et al.  Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation, by David Huron . Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 2006 , 2007 .

[32]  William E. Caplin,et al.  The Classical Cadence: Conceptions and Misconceptions , 2004 .

[33]  David R. W. Sears,et al.  Expecting the end: Continuous expectancy ratings for tonal cadences , 2018, Psychology of Music.

[34]  T. Sørensen,et al.  Is threat the only modulator of attentional selectivity? Redefining the Easterbrook hypothesis , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[35]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  The Role of Expectation and Probabilistic Learning in Auditory Boundary Perception: A Model Comparison , 2010, Perception.

[36]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Modularity in musical processing: the automaticity of harmonic priming. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  M. Pearce,et al.  Predictive uncertainty in auditory sequence processing , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[38]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Multi-Timescale Perceptual History Resolves Visual Ambiguity , 2008, PloS one.

[39]  E. Bigand,et al.  Perceiving musical tension in long chord sequences , 1999, Psychological research.

[40]  C. Krumhansl Music as Cognition. , 1987 .

[41]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  Sanford Weisberg,et al.  An R Companion to Applied Regression , 2010 .

[43]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Implicit learning of tonality: a self-organizing approach. , 2000, Psychological review.

[44]  William Earl Caplin,et al.  Classical Form: A Theory of Formal Functions for the Instrumental Music of Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven , 1998 .

[45]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  Methods for Combining Statistical Models of Music , 2004, CMMR.

[46]  A. Bregman,et al.  Auditory stream segregation and the control of dissonance in polyphonic music , 1987 .

[47]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Priming in melody perception: tracking down the strength of cognitive expectations. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  David Margulis,et al.  Musical expectancy and thrills , 2011 .

[49]  Robert O. Gjerdingen,et al.  The Cognition of Basic Musical Structures , 2004 .

[50]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  The representation of harmonic structure in music: Hierarchies of stability as a function of context , 1983, Cognition.

[51]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Auditory Expectations for Newly Acquired Structures , 2022 .

[52]  D. Sammler,et al.  Untangling syntactic and sensory processing: an ERP study of music perception. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[53]  E. Terhardt Pitch, consonance, and harmony. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[54]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Time course of chord priming , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[55]  M. Schmuckler Expectation in music: Investigation of melodic and harmonic processes. , 1989 .

[56]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Anchoring effects in music: The resolution of dissonance , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[57]  Charles Delbé,et al.  RECURRENT SELF-ORGANIZATION OF SENSORY SIGNALS IN THE AUDITORY DOMAIN , 2008 .

[58]  Albert S. Bregman,et al.  The Auditory Scene. (Book Reviews: Auditory Scene Analysis. The Perceptual Organization of Sound.) , 1990 .

[59]  L. Poundie Burstein The Half Cadence and Other Such Slippery Events , 2014 .

[60]  E. Terhardt,et al.  Pitch of complex signals according to virtual‐pitch theory: Tests, examples, and predictions , 1982 .

[61]  M. Leman An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in probe-tone ratings. , 2000 .

[62]  M. Neuwirth,et al.  What Is a Cadence?: Theoretical and Analytical Perspectives on Cadences in the Classical Repertoire , 2015 .

[63]  T. Jaeger,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[64]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  The Role of Harmonic Expectancy Violations in Musical Emotions: Evidence from Subjective, Physiological, and Neural Responses , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  H. Bergh,et al.  Examples of Mixed-Effects Modeling with Crossed Random Effects and with Binomial Data. , 2008 .

[66]  María Herrojo Ruiz,et al.  Unsupervised statistical learning underpins computational, behavioural, and neural manifestations of musical expectation , 2010, NeuroImage.

[67]  B. Tillmann,et al.  The tonal function of a task-irrelevant chord modulates speed of visual processing , 2008, Cognition.

[68]  Roland Eberlein,et al.  A Method of Analysing Harmony, Based on Interval Patterns or "Gestalten" , 1996, Joint International Conference on Cognitive and Systematic Musicology.

[69]  A. Friederici,et al.  Brain Indices of Music Processing: Nonmusicians are Musical , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  W. Dowling Emotion and Meaning in Music , 2008 .

[71]  Diana Deutsch,et al.  THE PROCESSING OF PITCH COMBINATIONS , 1999 .

[72]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  EXPECTATION IN MELODY: THE INFLUENCE OF CONTEXT AND LEARNING , 2006 .

[73]  Bruno Cozzi,et al.  Expression of calcium-binding proteins and selected neuropeptides in the human, chimpanzee, and crab-eating macaque claustrum , 2014, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[74]  Matthias Abend Cognitive Foundations Of Musical Pitch , 2016 .

[75]  Christine Nadel,et al.  Explaining Psychological Statistics , 2016 .

[76]  Jeffrey L. Birk,et al.  Tonal centers and expectancy: facilitation or inhibition of chords at the top of the harmonic hierarchy? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[77]  Søren Højsgaard,et al.  A Kenward-Roger approximation and parametric bootstrap methods for tests in linear mixed models: The R Package pbkrtest , 2014 .

[78]  F. A. Gore Ouseley,et al.  A Treatise on Harmony , 2008 .

[79]  E. Rosch Cognitive reference points , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[80]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  An auditory analogue of the sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage , 1972 .

[81]  Marc Leman,et al.  The Cortical Topography of Tonal Structures Underlying Western Music , 2002, Science.

[82]  M. Pearce,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of melodic processing in congenital amusia , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[83]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Implicit knowledge versus psychoacoustic similarity in priming of chords , 1998 .