The Committee Charge, Framing Interpersonal Agreement, and Consensus Models of Group Quantitative Judgment

Abstract Procedural mechanisms, ostensibly serving to facilitate the consensus process in group decision making, have also been observed to influence process and outcome, frequently in unexpected and undesirable ways. Procedural influence in this study was attributable to the initial enabling charge that focused the group consensus process on task performance, interpersonal relations, or nothing. Both task- and interpersonally oriented groups allocated less money on average to an AIDS awareness educational program than did un-charged groups, and groups generally allocated less money than independent individuals who worked alone. Explanation focused on discussion as norm enhancing and committee charge as further intensifying norm salience, albeit in a somewhat unexpected direction. A “social judgment scheme” model based on the pattern of member preferences, and member positions in that pattern, accurately predicted group quantitative judgments within each experimental condition, whereas plausible alternative models and baselines did not. Among other findings, post-group member confidence in the decision was higher than that of parallel individuals, and highest among those from “task-oriented” groups; the latter also were more willing than ex-members of “interpersonally oriented” groups to discriminate among fellow members in retrospectively evaluating the relative contributions of each other.

[1]  Wing Tung Au,et al.  Effects of group size and procedural influence on consensual judgments of quantity: The example of damage awards and mock civil juries , 1997 .

[2]  Rand R. Wilcox,et al.  Some practical reasons for reconsidering the Kolmogorov‐Smirnov test , 1997 .

[3]  J. H. Davis,et al.  Procedural Influence on Group Decision Making: The Case of Straw Polls—Observation and Simulation , 1996 .

[4]  J. H. Davis,et al.  Group decision making and quantitative judgments: A consensus model. , 1996 .

[5]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Dynamical Systems in Social Psychology , 1994 .

[6]  J. H. Davis,et al.  Quantitative Decisions by Groups and Individuals: Voting Procedures and Monetary Awards by Mock Civil Juries , 1993 .

[7]  R. Litan Verdict: Assessing the Civil Jury System , 1993 .

[8]  J. H. Davis,et al.  Some compelling intuitions about group consensus decisions, theoretical and empirical research, and interpersonal aggregation phenomena: Selected examples 1950-1990 , 1992 .

[9]  S. Galam,et al.  Towards a theory of collective phenomena: Consensus and attitude changes in groups , 1991 .

[10]  Carl A. Kallgren,et al.  A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: A Theoretical Refinement and Reevaluation of the Role of Norms in Human Behavior , 1991 .

[11]  Norman H. Anderson,et al.  Contributions to information integration theory , 1991 .

[12]  Tatsuya Kameda,et al.  Some social mechanics of group decision making: The distribution of opinion, polling sequence, and implications for consensus. , 1989 .

[13]  J. H. Davis Psychology and law: The last 15 years. , 1989 .

[14]  Norbert L. Kerr,et al.  Influence processes and consensus models in decision-making groups. , 1989 .

[15]  J. H. Davis,et al.  Effects of straw polls on group decision making: Sequential voting pattern, timing, and local majorities. , 1988 .

[16]  N. Kerr,et al.  Asymmetric influence in mock jury deliberation: jurors' bias for leniency. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  R. Shepard,et al.  Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. , 1987, Science.

[18]  M. S. Poole,et al.  Communication and Group Decision-Making , 1986 .

[19]  N. Kerr,et al.  The effects of jury size and polling method on the process and product of jury deliberation. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  William R. Ferrell,et al.  Combining Individual Judgments , 1985 .

[21]  Verlin B. Hinsz,et al.  Order Effects in Multiple Decisions by Groups: A Demonstration With Mock Juries and Trial Procedures , 1984 .

[22]  N. Pennington,et al.  Inside the Jury. , 1985 .

[23]  Garold Stasser,et al.  Group decision making and social influence: A social interaction sequence model. , 1981 .

[24]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[25]  J. L. Myers,et al.  Group dynamics and individual performances: Pseudogroup and quasi-F analyses. , 1981 .

[26]  C. Wagner,et al.  Rational Consensus in Science and Society , 1981 .

[27]  Paul B. Paulus,et al.  Psychology of Group Influence , 1981 .

[28]  N. Anderson Foundations of information integration theory , 1981 .

[29]  Joel Ager,et al.  Analysis of Variance in Small Group Research , 1978 .

[30]  C. Plott,et al.  A Model of Agenda Influence on Committee Decisions , 1978 .

[31]  M. Degroot Reaching a Consensus , 1974 .

[32]  J. H. Davis Group decision and social interaction: A theory of social decision schemes. , 1973 .

[33]  George Gallup,et al.  The Gallup Poll; Public Opinion, 1935-1971 , 1972 .

[34]  K. R. Hammond,et al.  Cognitive conflict between persons: Application of the “lens model” paradigm , 1966 .

[35]  Harold Gulliksen,et al.  Contributions to mathematical psychology , 1964 .

[36]  P. Suppes,et al.  Mathematical methods in small group processes , 1963 .

[37]  E. Thomas,et al.  Models of group problem solving. , 1961, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[38]  Dorwin Cartwright,et al.  Studies in Social Power. , 1960 .

[39]  J. Thibaut,et al.  Psychological set and social conformity. , 1956, Journal of personality.

[40]  J. French,et al.  A formal theory of social power. , 1956, Psychological review.

[41]  Herbert Solomon,et al.  Two models of group behavior in the solution of eureka-type problems , 1955 .

[42]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  A Formal Theory of Interaction in Social Groups , 1952 .

[43]  R. Lippitt,et al.  Leadership and Isolation. , 1943 .