Cross-language activation of morphological relatives in cognates: the role of orthographic overlap and task-related processing
暂无分享,去创建一个
Ton Dijkstra | R. Harald Baayen | Kimberley Mulder | R. Baayen | T. Dijkstra | Kimberley Mulder | R. Harald Baayen | Minna Lehtonen
[1] R. Baayen,et al. Reading polymorphemic Dutch compounds: toward a multiple route model of lexical processing. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] D. Balota,et al. Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[3] ROCHELLE LIEBER,et al. The morphological complexity of simplex nouns , 1997 .
[4] Alan Y. Chiang,et al. Generalized Additive Models: An Introduction With R , 2007, Technometrics.
[5] Austin F. Frank,et al. Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R , 2010 .
[6] T. Dijkstra,et al. On being blinded by your other language: Effects of task demands on interlingual homograph recognition , 2000 .
[7] Barbara J. Juhasz,et al. Effects of morphological families on English compound word recognition: A multitask investigation , 2011 .
[8] T. Dijkstra,et al. The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision , 2002, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[9] T. Dijkstra,et al. Interlingual homograph recognition: Effects of task demands and language intermixing , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[10] R. Baayen,et al. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .
[11] Robert Schreuder,et al. Effects of Family Size for Complex Words , 2000 .
[12] W. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Productivity and priming: Morphemic decomposition in Arabic , 2011 .
[13] R. Harald Baayen,et al. Morphological dynamics in compound processing , 2008 .
[14] L. Feldman. Modeling Morphological Processing , 2013 .
[15] Robert Schreuder,et al. How Complex Simplex Words can be , 1997 .
[16] L. Feldman,et al. The role of component function in visual recognition of Chinese characters. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[17] A. Dijkstra. Bilingual Visual Word Recognition and Lexical Access. , 2005 .
[18] Nivja H. de Jong,et al. The morphological family size effect and morphology , 2000 .
[19] Jon Andoni Duñabeitia,et al. Orthographic and associative neighborhood density effects: what is shared, what is different? , 2010, Psychophysiology.
[20] R. Harald Baayen,et al. Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R, 1st Edition , 2008 .
[21] Robert Schreuder,et al. Morphological resonance in the mental lexicon , 2003 .
[22] R. Baayen,et al. Morphological family size in a morphologically rich language: the case of Finnish compared with Dutch and Hebrew. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[23] R. Baayen,et al. Effects of primary and secondary morphological family size in monolingual and bilingual word processing , 2014 .
[24] Robert Schreuder,et al. The Processing and Representation of Dutch and English Compounds: Peripheral Morphological and Central Orthographic Effects , 2002, Brain and Language.
[25] Robert Schreuder,et al. Morphological family size in a morphologically rich language : The case of Finnish compared to Dutch and Hebrew . Morphological Family Size in Finnish Fermı́n , 2004 .
[26] R. Baayen,et al. How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition , 2010 .
[27] Marcus Taft,et al. Orthographic Influences When Processing Spoken Pseudowords: Theoretical Implications , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[28] R. H. Baayen,et al. The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .
[29] Nivja H. de Jong,et al. Changing places: A cross-language perspective on frequency and family size in Dutch and Hebrew , 2005 .
[30] A.F.J. Dijkstra,et al. The multilingual lexicon , 2007 .
[31] J. Grainger,et al. An Electrophysiological Study of the Effects of Orthographic Neighborhood Size on Printed Word Perception , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] S. Wood. Generalized Additive Models: An Introduction with R , 2006 .
[33] Vladimir I. Levenshtein,et al. Binary codes capable of correcting deletions, insertions, and reversals , 1965 .
[34] De Jong,et al. Morphological families in the mental lexicon , 2002 .
[35] Noëlle Font,et al. Rôle de la langue dans l'accès au lexique chez les bilingues : influence de la proximité orthographique et sémantique interlangue sur la reconnaissance visuelle de mots , 2001 .
[36] J Grainger,et al. Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.
[37] Koenraad De Smedt,et al. Markedness effects in Norwegian–English bilinguals: Task-dependent use of language-specific letters and bigrams , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[38] R. Schreuder,et al. A roommate in cream: Morphological family size effects on interlingual homograph recognition , 2005 .
[39] R. Schreuder,et al. Morphological Family Size effects in L1 and L2 processing: An electrophysiological study , 2013 .
[40] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[41] Rebecca Treiman,et al. The English Lexicon Project , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[42] Cristina Burani,et al. Visual processing of Italian verbs and adjectives: The role of the inflectional family size , 2003 .
[43] Jasone Cenoz,et al. The multilingual lexicon , 2003 .