Space and Time in Perception and Action: Bridging the gap: a model of common neural mechanisms underlying the Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag effect, and the representational momentum effect
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Ruben Budelli,et al. Spatial facilitation is involved in flash-lag effect , 2007, Vision Research.
[2] Markus Lappe,et al. A model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process , 2000, Vision Research.
[3] C. Gilbert,et al. Perceptual learning and top-down influences in primary visual cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] K. Kreegipuu,et al. Perceived onset time and position of a moving stimulus , 2003, Vision Research.
[5] V. Bringuier,et al. Horizontal propagation of visual activity in the synaptic integration field of area 17 neurons. , 1999, Science.
[6] Rodney J. Douglas,et al. Feedback interactions between neuronal pointers and maps for attentional processing , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[7] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Compensation of neural delays in visual‐motor behaviour: No evidence for shorter afferent delays for visual motion , 2004 .
[8] P. Cavanagh,et al. Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli , 2000, Vision Research.
[9] T J Sejnowski,et al. Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.
[10] J. Bharucha,et al. Judged displacement in apparent vertical and horizontal motion , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] Peter Dayan,et al. Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems , 2001 .
[12] Michael J. Berry,et al. Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina , 1999, Nature.
[13] Wolfram Erlhagen,et al. The role of action plans and other cognitive factors in motion extrapolation: A modelling study , 2004 .
[14] A. Grinvald,et al. Real-time optical imaging of naturally evoked electrical activity in intact frog brain , 1984, Nature.
[15] Amir C. Akhavan,et al. Parametric Population Representation of Retinal Location: Neuronal Interaction Dynamics in Cat Primary Visual Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] Haim Sompolinsky,et al. Traveling Waves and the Processing of Weakly Tuned Inputs in a Cortical Network Module , 2004, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.
[17] W. Metzger,et al. Versuch einer gemeinsamen Theorie der Phänomene Fröhlichs und Hazelhoffs und Kritik ihrer Verfahren zur Messung der Empfindungszeit , 1932 .
[18] C. Koch,et al. Recurrent excitation in neocortical circuits , 1995, Science.
[19] David Whitney,et al. Temporal facilitation for moving stimuli is independent of changes in direction , 2000, Vision Research.
[20] Emilio Salinas,et al. Vector reconstruction from firing rates , 1994, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.
[21] David Whitney,et al. The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[22] Nestor Caticha,et al. Computational neurobiology of the flash-lag effect , 2005, Vision Research.
[23] Romi Nijhawan,et al. Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.
[24] Kuno Kirschfeld,et al. The Fröhlich effect: a consequence of the interaction of visual focal attention and metacontrast , 1999, Vision Research.
[25] M. Lappe,et al. Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.
[26] Sonja Stork,et al. Comparing mislocalizations with moving stimuli: The Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag, and representational momentum , 2002 .
[27] Y Dan,et al. Motion-Induced Perceptual Extrapolation of Blurred Visual Targets , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] G Aschersleben,et al. Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: The Fröhlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[29] V. Lamme,et al. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[30] P. Lennie. The physiological basis of variations in visual latency , 1981, Vision Research.
[31] Gopathy Purushothaman,et al. Moving ahead through differential visual latency , 1998, Nature.
[32] Martin A. Giese,et al. Exact solution of the nonlinear dynamics of recurrent neural mechanisms for direction selectivity , 2002, Neurocomputing.
[33] J. Cowan,et al. A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue , 1973, Kybernetik.
[34] Wolfram Erlhagen,et al. Internal models for visual perception , 2003, Biological Cybernetics.
[35] T. Hubbard,et al. Does representational momentum reflect a distortion of the length or the endpoint of a trajectory? , 2002, Cognition.
[36] M. Coltheart,et al. Iconic memory and visible persistence , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] G. Schöner,et al. The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations , 1999, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[38] Stanley A. Klein,et al. Extrapolation or attention shift? , 1995, Nature.
[39] D. Fitzpatrick. Seeing beyond the receptive field in primary visual cortex , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[40] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Nonlinear neural networks: Principles, mechanisms, and architectures , 1988, Neural Networks.
[41] T. Hubbard. Cognitive representation of linear motion: Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement , 1990, Memory & cognition.
[42] Friedrich W. Fröhlich,et al. Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit , 1924, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.
[43] Ian M Thornton,et al. The onset repulsion effect. , 2002, Spatial vision.