Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space

With every rapid gaze shift (saccade), our eyes experience a different view of the world. Stable perception of visual space requires that points in the new image are associated with corresponding points in the previous image. The brain may use an extraretinal eye position signal to compensate for gaze changes, or, alternatively, exploit the image contents to determine associated locations. Support for a uniform extraretinal signal comes from findings that the apparent position of objects briefly flashed around the time of a saccade is often shifted in the direction of the saccade. This view is challenged, however, by observations that the magnitude and direction of the displacement varies across the visual field. Led by the observation that non-uniform displacements typically occurred in studies conducted in slightly illuminated rooms, here we determine the dependence of perisaccadic mislocalization on the availability of visual spatial references at various times around a saccade. We find that presaccadic compression occurs only if visual references are available immediately after, rather than before or during, the saccade. Our findings indicate that the visual processes of transsaccadic spatial localization use mainly postsaccadic visual information.

[1]  Christopher J Bockisch,et al.  Different motor systems use similar damped extraretinal eye position information , 1999, Vision Research.

[2]  L. Matin,et al.  Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[3]  M. C. Whitehead,et al.  Development of fungiform papillae, taste buds, and their innervation in the hamster , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[4]  K. Gannon,et al.  Transduction of bitter and sweet taste by gustducin , 1996, Nature.

[5]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  H. Ogawa,et al.  Response properties of macaque monkey chorda tympani fibers , 1975, The Journal of general physiology.

[7]  D. M. MACKAY,et al.  Mislocation of Test Flashes during Saccadic Image Displacements , 1970, Nature.

[8]  Paul Dassonville,et al.  The use of egocentric and exocentric location cues in saccadic programming , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  Yngve Zotterman,et al.  Action potentials in the glossopharyngeal nerve and in the chorda tympani1 , 1935 .

[10]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Retinal versus extraretinal influences in flash localization during saccadic eye movements in the presence of a visible background , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  D. Mcburney,et al.  Temperature dependence of human taste responses. , 1973, Physiology & behavior.

[12]  J. Rodin,et al.  Effects of temperature on the perceived sweetness of sucrose , 1982, Physiology & Behavior.

[13]  B. Green,et al.  Derivation and evaluation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties , 1993 .

[14]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  R P Erickson,et al.  Duplexity Theory of Taste. , 1965, Science.

[16]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Perceived geometrical relationships affected by eye-movement signals , 1997, Nature.

[17]  Bruce Oakley,et al.  Taste responses of human chorda tympani nerve , 1985 .

[18]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy , 1998, Vision Research.

[19]  N. Bischof,et al.  [Investigations and considerations of directional perception during voluntary saccadic eye movements]. , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[20]  H. Honda Saccade-contingent displacement of the apparent position of visual stimuli flashed on a dimly illuminated structured background , 1993, Vision Research.

[21]  S. Travers,et al.  Responsiveness of neurons in the hamster parabrachial nuclei to taste mixtures , 1984, The Journal of general physiology.

[22]  M. C. Whitehead,et al.  Organization of geniculate and trigeminal ganglion cells innervating single fungiform taste papillae: a study with tetramethylrhodamine dextran amine labeling , 1999, Neuroscience.

[23]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[24]  N. Bischof,et al.  Untersuchungen und Überlegungen zur Richtungswahrnehmung bei willkürlichen sakkadischen Augenbewegungen , 1968 .

[25]  C. Bockisch,et al.  Where are the things we see? , 1997, Nature.

[26]  B. Green,et al.  The effect of cooling the tongue on the perceived intensity of taste , 1987 .

[27]  Makoto Nakamura,et al.  Temperature dependence of amiloride-sensitive and -insensitive components of rat taste nerve response to NaCl , 1988, Brain Research.

[28]  N. Ryba,et al.  Putative Mammalian Taste Receptors A Class of Taste-Specific GPCRs with Distinct Topographic Selectivity , 1999, Cell.

[29]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[30]  H. Ogawa,et al.  Multiple sensitivity of chorda tympani fibres of the rat and hamster to gustatory and thermal stimuli , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  Markus Lappe,et al.  A model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process , 2000, Vision Research.