Emergent Filling In Induced by Motion Integration Reveals a High-Level Mechanism in Filling In

The visual system is intelligent—it is capable of recovering a coherent surface from an incomplete one, a feat known as perceptual completion or filling in. Traditionally, it has been assumed that surface features are interpolated in a way that resembles the fragmented parts. Using displays featuring four circular apertures, we showed in the study reported here that a distinct completed feature (horizontal motion) arises from local ones (oblique motions)—we term this process emergent filling in. Adaptation to emergent filling-in motion generated a dynamic motion aftereffect that was not due to spreading of local motion from the isolated apertures. The filling-in motion aftereffect occurred in both modal and amodal completions, and it was modulated by selective attention. These findings highlight the importance of high-level interpolation processes in filling in and are consistent with the idea that during emergent filling in, the more cognitive-symbolic processes in later areas (e.g., the middle temporal visual area and the lateral occipital complex) provide important feedback signals to guide more isomorphic processes in earlier areas (V1 and V2).

[1]  H. Komatsu The neural mechanisms of perceptual filling-in , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Nishida,et al.  Complete interocular transfer of motion aftereffect with flickering test , 1994, Vision Research.

[3]  A. Michotte,et al.  Les compléments amodaux des structures perceptives , 1964 .

[4]  N. Weisstein,et al.  A phantom-motion aftereffect. , 1977, Science.

[5]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  The primary visual cortex fills in color , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  R. Gregory,et al.  Cognitive Contours , 1972, Nature.

[7]  Jon Driver,et al.  Parallel detection of Kanizsa subjective figures in the human visual system , 1994, Nature.

[8]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Spatial integration and cortical dynamics. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  Frank Tong,et al.  Filling-in of visual phantoms in the human brain , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Masking unveils pre-amodal completion representation in visual search , 2001, Nature.

[11]  Luiz Pessoa,et al.  Filling-in is for finding out , 1998 .

[12]  A. Metha,et al.  Enhanced motion aftereffect for complex motions , 1999, Vision Research.

[13]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Surfaces versus features in visual search , 1992, Nature.

[14]  P. Mazzoni,et al.  Cross-fixation transfer of motion aftereffects with expansion motion , 2006, Vision Research.

[15]  Eric Halgren,et al.  Cortical activation to illusory shapes as measured with magnetoencephalography , 2003, NeuroImage.

[16]  Peter De Weerd,et al.  Responses of cells in monkey visual cortex during perceptual filling-in of an artificial scotoma , 1995, Nature.

[17]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Perceptual Completion across the Vertical Meridian and the Role of Early Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[18]  Stephen E. Palmer,et al.  Late influences on perceptual grouping: Amodal completion , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Jon Driver,et al.  Spreading of Visual Attention to Modally Versus Amodally Completed Regions , 1997 .

[20]  G. Kanizsa,et al.  Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Perception , 1979 .

[21]  M. Seghier,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging Findings on the Human Perception of Illusory Contours , 2022 .

[22]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Early completion of occluded objects , 1998, Vision Research.

[23]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  P Tynan,et al.  Moving visual phantoms: a new contour completion effect. , 1975, Science.

[25]  H. Gerrits,et al.  Simultaneous contrast, filling-in process and information processing in man's visual system , 1970, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Motion Integration of Individual Neurons in the Middle Temporal Visual Area , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  C. Hung,et al.  Cortical processing of a brightness illusion. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Jon Driver,et al.  Kanizsa subjective figures can act as occluding surfaces at parallel stages of visual search , 1998 .

[29]  L. Reijnders,et al.  Higher Order Effects , 1992 .

[30]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Preattentive Filling-in of Visual Surfaces in Parietal Extinction , 1997, Science.

[31]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[32]  P. Kellman,et al.  A theory of visual interpolation in object perception , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  D Marr,et al.  Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[34]  ANDREW SMITH,et al.  Tilt aftereffects with subjective contours , 1975, Nature.

[35]  Manish Singh Modal and Amodal Completion Generate Different Shapes , 2004, Psychological science.

[36]  Y Kamitani,et al.  Afterimage of Perceptually Filled-in Surface , 2001, Science.

[37]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.

[38]  S Grossberg,et al.  3-D vision and figure-ground separation by visual cortex , 2010, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[40]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  A Neural Model of Contour Integration in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1998, Neural Computation.

[41]  N. Rubin,et al.  fMRI Activation in Response to Illusory Contours and Salient Regions in the Human Lateral Occipital Complex , 2003, Neuron.

[42]  J. Lorenceau,et al.  Directional shifts in the barber pole illusion: Effects of spatial frequency, spatial adaptation, and lateral masking , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.

[43]  R Gattass,et al.  Dynamic surrounds of receptive fields in primate striate cortex: a physiological basis for perceptual completion? , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  M Dojat,et al.  Moving illusory contours activate primary visual cortex: an fMRI study. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  C. Gilbert Horizontal integration and cortical dynamics , 1992, Neuron.

[46]  John A. Greenwood,et al.  Tuning properties of radial phantom motion aftereffects , 2004, Vision Research.

[47]  S. Ullman,et al.  Filling-in the gaps: The shape of subjective contours and a model for their generation , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.

[48]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .

[49]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Neuronal Mechanisms for Illusory Brightness Perception in Humans , 2005, Neuron.

[50]  Daniel C. Dennett,et al.  Filling in versus finding out: A ubiquitous confusion in cognitive science , 1992 .

[51]  A. Dale,et al.  The Representation of Illusory and Real Contours in Human Cortical Visual Areas Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  T. S. Lee,et al.  Dynamics of subjective contour formation in the early visual cortex. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  No Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence for Brightness and Color Filling-In In Early Human Visual Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  A. Lak,et al.  Attention during adaptation weakens negative afterimages of perceptually colour-spread surfaces. , 2008, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[55]  Barry D. Vaughan,et al.  Object-Based Visual Selection: Evidence From Perceptual Completion , 1998 .

[56]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Setting Boundaries: Brain Dynamics of Modal and Amodal Illusory Shape Completion in Humans , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  R. Snowden,et al.  Phantom motion aftereffects – evidence of detectors for the analysis of optic flow , 1997, Current Biology.