ASSESSING EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON FORAGING BEHAVIOR OF MULE DEER

Abstract We applied optimal foraging theory to test effects of habitat and predation risk on foraging behavior of mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus) subject to predation by mountain lions (Puma concolor). We predicted that deer would spend less time foraging, have higher giving-up densities of food (GUDs), and have higher vigilance behavior when occupying patch edges than when in open and forest interiors. We also measured GUDs in 3 microhabitats within 3 forest types. We used pellet-group surveys to estimate habitat and microhabitat use, and we assessed vigilance behavior with automatic camera systems. The GUDs (perceived predation risk) were greater in forests of Douglas fir (Pseudostuga menziensii) than mountain mahogany (Cercocarpus ledifolius). In forests of Douglas fir, GUDs were greatest in the forest interior, declined at the forest edge, and were lowest in the open microhabitat. Microhabitat features did not influence GUDs in the mountain mahogany forest. Pellet-group data indicated more activity in the open than in the edge or forest. Based on photographs, deer were more vigilant at forest edges than in open and forest areas. We concluded that deer are responding to predation risk by biasing their feeding efforts at the scale of habitats and microhabitats and altering their habitat-specific patterns of vigilance behavior.

[1]  S. Carthew,et al.  Monitoring animal activity with automated photography , 1991 .

[2]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus , 1990 .

[3]  W. Cresswell,et al.  Flocking is an effective anti-predation strategy in redshanks, Tringa totanus , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  T. J. Breen,et al.  Biostatistical Analysis (2nd ed.). , 1986 .

[5]  R. Macarthur,et al.  Graphical Representation and Stability Conditions of Predator-Prey Interactions , 1963, The American Naturalist.

[6]  Burt P. Kotler,et al.  Applying patch use to assess aspects of foraging behavior in Nubian ibex , 1994 .

[7]  A Sih,et al.  Optimal behavior: can foragers balance two conflicting demands? , 1980, Science.

[8]  Paul Wilson Puma predation on guanacos in Torres del Paine National Park, Chile , 1984 .

[9]  J. Skinner,et al.  Vigilance Behaviour in African Ungulates: The Role of Predation Pressure , 1998 .

[10]  Clyde W. Neu,et al.  A TECHNIQUE FOR ANALYSIS OF UTILIZATION- AVAILABILITY DATA' , 1974 .

[11]  D. Fraser,et al.  Experimental Evaluation of Predator-Prey Relationships in a Patchy Environment: Consequences For Habitat Use Patterns in Minnows , 1982 .

[12]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  Patch use predation risk. II : A test with fox squirrels, Sciurus niger , 1992 .

[13]  A. Sinclair,et al.  Population Consequences of Predation-Sensitive Foraging: The Serengeti Wildebeest , 1995 .

[14]  Burt P. Kotler Patch use by gerbils in a risky environment: manipulating food and safety to test four models , 1997 .

[15]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: Foraging under predation risk , 1999 .

[16]  Peter Arcese,et al.  Serengeti II : dynamics, management, and conservation of an ecosystem , 1996 .

[17]  Colin W. Clark,et al.  Antipredator behavior and the asset-protection principle , 1994 .

[18]  M. Hornocker,et al.  Analysis of mountain lion predation upon mule deer and elk in the Idaho Primitive area , 1967 .

[19]  Burt P. Kotler,et al.  Predation and competition: the interaction of two types of species interactions , 1989 .

[20]  Burt P. Kotler,et al.  The influence of snakes on the foraging behavior of gerbils , 1993 .

[21]  A. Houston,et al.  Evolutionarily stable levels of vigilance as a function of group size , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  K. J. Constan Winter Foods and Range Use of Three Species of Ungulates , 1972 .

[23]  W. B. Collins,et al.  Feeding behavior and habitat selection of mule deer and elk on northern Utah summer range , 1983 .

[24]  S. L. Lima Initiation and termination of daily feeding in dark-eyed juncos: influences of predation risk and energy reserves , 1988 .

[25]  Jerrold H. Zar,et al.  Field and laboratory methods for general ecology , 1984 .

[26]  F. James Rohlf,et al.  Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .

[27]  S. L. Lima Vigilance during the Initiation of Daily Feeding in Dark-Eyed Juncos , 1988 .

[28]  P. Galina-Tessaro,et al.  Mule deer density and pattern distribution in the pine-oak forest at the Sierra de La Laguna in Baja California Sur, Mexico , 1991 .

[29]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  The Ecology of Fear: Optimal Foraging, Game Theory, and Trophic Interactions , 1999 .

[30]  W. Collins,et al.  Habitat Preferences of Mule Deer Odocoileus-Hemionus as Rated by Pellet Group Distributions , 1981 .

[31]  R. Macarthur,et al.  On Optimal Use of a Patchy Environment , 1966, The American Naturalist.

[32]  D. J. Hall,et al.  An Experimental Test of the Effects of Predation Risk on Habitat Use in Fish , 1983 .

[33]  R. Terry Bowyer,et al.  Costs and Benefits of Group Living in a Recently Social Ungulate: The Alaskan Moose , 1994 .

[34]  V. Bleich Mountain Sheep and Coyotes: Patterns of Predator Evasion in a Mountain Ungulate , 1999 .