Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill

There is disagreement about the effectiveness of observational practice for the acquisition of novel coordination skills and the type of processes involved in observation of novel movements. In this study, we examined learning of a bimanual 90° phase offset through comparisons of three groups; physical practice, observational practice and no practice (n = 12/group). Groups were compared before and after practice on perception and production scans of the practised pattern. The observation group was yoked to the physical group such that observers watched repeated demonstrations of a learning model. Although there were no positive effects of observational practice for physical performance measures, the observation group did not differ from the physical practice group and was more accurate than controls on perceptual discrimination measures after practice. We concluded that observation of a novel bimanual movement can aid perception but that physical practice is necessary for immediate physical performance benefits. These results are discussed in terms of cognitive mediation models of motor skill learning.

[1]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Learning as a function of coordination bias: building upon pre-practice behaviours. , 2002, Human movement science.

[2]  G Atkinson,et al.  The ecological validity of laboratory cycling: Does body size explain the difference between laboratory- and field-based cycling performance? , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[3]  D L Wright,et al.  Learning and Transfer of a Relative Phase Pattern and a Joint Amplitude Ratio in a Rhythmic Multijoint Arm Movement , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  Ian M. Franks,et al.  Attention focusing instructions and coordination bias : implications for learning a novel bimanual task , 2000 .

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.

[7]  G Wulf,et al.  Physical and Observational Practice Afford Unique Learning Opportunities , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .

[9]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Learning and transfer as dynamical paradigms for behavioral change , 1992 .

[10]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  C. Heyes,et al.  Motor learning by observation: Evidence from a serial reaction time task , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[12]  David L Wright,et al.  Learning to Detect Error in Movement Timing Using Physical and Observational Practice , 2005, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[13]  Robert R. Horn,et al.  Observational learning : Is it time we took another look? , 2004 .

[14]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Evaluation of scanning methodology in bimanual coordination. , 2005, Motor control.

[15]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Effects of manipulating relative and absolute motion information during observational learning of an aiming task , 2001, Journal of sports sciences.

[17]  John J Buchanan,et al.  Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice , 2010, Psychological research.

[18]  S. Bennett,et al.  Observational Modeling Effects for Movement Dynamics and Movement Outcome Measures Across Differing Task Constraints: A Meta-Analysis , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Feedback Effects on Learning a Novel Bimanual Coordination Pattern: Support for the Guidance Hypothesis , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Motor Learning Through Observation , 2010 .

[21]  L. Craighero,et al.  Electrophysiology of Action Representation , 2004, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[22]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[24]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[25]  J Zuckerman,et al.  Modeling effects on motor performance. , 1976, Research quarterly.

[26]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Contextual interference: single task versus multi-task learning. , 2004 .

[28]  S. Geisser,et al.  On methods in the analysis of profile data , 1959 .

[29]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Using scanning trials to assess intrinsic coordination dynamics , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[30]  P. Haggard,et al.  Experts see it all: configural effects in action observation , 2010, Psychological research.

[31]  David R. Collins,et al.  Perceptual coupling in rhythmic movement coordination: stable perception leads to stable action , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  A. Bandura Analysis of Modeling Processes. , 1975 .

[33]  Maureen R. Weiss,et al.  Modeling: Considerations for motor skill performance and psychological responses , 2001 .

[34]  A Bandura,et al.  Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: a causal analysis. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[35]  W. Prinz,et al.  The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .

[36]  Gavin Breslin,et al.  A comparison of intra- and inter-limb relative motion information in modelling a novel motor skill. , 2006, Human movement science.

[37]  Polemnia G. Amazeen,et al.  Is Dynamics the Content of a Generalized Motor Program for Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination? , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[38]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[39]  Gavin Breslin,et al.  Modelling relative motion to facilitate intra-limb coordination. , 2005, Human movement science.

[40]  J. Kelso,et al.  A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[41]  A Bandura,et al.  The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: making the unobservable observable. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[42]  David R. Collins,et al.  The effect of frequency on the visual perception of relative phase and phase variability of two oscillating objects , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  I M Franks,et al.  Learning a Coordination Skill: Interactive Effects of Instruction and Feedback , 2001, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[44]  Young U. Ryu,et al.  Observational practice of relative but not absolute motion features in a single-limb multi-joint coordination task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  Romeo Chua,et al.  The Role of Video in Facilitating Perception and Action of a Novel Coordination Movement , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[47]  Stefan Vogt The Imitative Mind: Visuomotor couplings in object-oriented and imitative actions , 2002 .

[48]  S. Vogt,et al.  From visuo-motor interactions to imitation learning: Behavioural and brain imaging studies , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[49]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  Gavin Breslin,et al.  Effect of Information Load and Time on Observational Learning , 2009, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[51]  D L Wright,et al.  Can Observational Practice Facilitate Error Recognition and Movement Production? , 2000, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[52]  A. Williams,et al.  What is modelled during observational learning? , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[53]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning a single limb multijoint coordination pattern: the impact of a mechanical constraint on the coordination dynamics of learning and transfer , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  P G Zanone,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[57]  U. Castiello,et al.  The Human Premotor Cortex Is 'Mirror' Only for Biological Actions , 2004, Current Biology.

[58]  Paul L. Gribble,et al.  Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation to the Primary Motor Cortex Interferes with Motor Learning by Observing , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  Ana Solodkin,et al.  Imaging motor imagery: methodological issues related to expertise. , 2008, Methods.

[60]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. , 1995 .