The minimal unit of phonological encoding: prosodic or lexical word

Wheeldon and Lahiri (Journal of Memory and Language 37 (1997) 356) used a prepared speech production task (Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117-152). New York: Academic Press; Sternberg, S., Wright, C. E., Knoll, R. L., & Monsell, S. (1980). Motor programs in rapid speech: additional evidence. In R. A. Cole (Ed.), The perception and production of fluent speech (pp. 507-534). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum) to demonstrate that the latency to articulate a sentence is a function of the number of phonological words it comprises. Latencies for the sentence [Ik zoek het] [water] 'I seek the water' were shorter than latencies for sentences like [Ik zoek] [vers] [water] 'I seek fresh water'. We extend this research by examining the prepared production of utterances containing phonological words that are less than a lexical word in length. Dutch compounds (e.g. ooglid 'eyelid') form a single morphosyntactic word and a phonological word, which in turn includes two phonological words. We compare their prepared production latencies to those syntactic phrases consisting of an adjective and a noun (e.g. oud lid 'old member') which comprise two morphosyntactic and two phonological words, and to morphologically simple words (e.g. orgel 'organ') which comprise one morphosyntactic and one phonological word. Our findings demonstrate that the effect is limited to phrasal level phonological words, suggesting that production models need to make a distinction between lexical and phrasal phonology.

[1]  W. Levelt Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations , 1992, Cognition.

[2]  Harry van der Hulst,et al.  The Structure of phonological representations , 1982 .

[3]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Implicit speech: Mechanism in perceptual encoding? , 1970 .

[4]  K. P. Mohanan,et al.  The Theory of Lexical Phonology , 1982 .

[5]  Linda Wheeldon Competitive processes during word-form encoding , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  Stuart T. Klapp,et al.  Syllable-dependent pronunciation latencies in number naming: A replication. , 1974 .

[7]  Draga Zec,et al.  The Phonology-syntax connection , 1990 .

[8]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The Latency and Duration of Rapid Movement Sequences: Comparisons of Speech and Typewriting , 1978 .

[9]  B. Dresher THE PROSODIC BASIS OF THE TIBERIAN HEBREW SYSTEM OF ACCENTS , 1994 .

[10]  Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi,et al.  Where is the length effect? A cross-linguistic study. , 1998 .

[11]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. , 1991 .

[12]  Carlos Gussenhoven,et al.  The Dutch foot and the chanted call , 1993, Journal of Linguistics.

[13]  H. Schriefers Phonological Facilitation in the Production of Two-word Utterances , 1999 .

[14]  Robert L. Mason,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 2003 .

[15]  G. Booij The Phonology of Dutch , 1995 .

[16]  Algemene Vereniging voor Taalwetenschap,et al.  Linguistics in the Netherlands, 1985 , 1986 .

[17]  A. Lahiri,et al.  Prosodic Units in Speech Production , 1997 .

[18]  B. Hayes Precompiled Phrasal Phonology , 2022 .

[19]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[20]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  G. Stelmach Information processing in motor control and learning , 1978 .

[22]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  Implicit speech in reading: Reconsidered , 1973 .

[23]  W. Levelt,et al.  Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? , 1994, Cognition.