Social information use is a process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Mikko Mönkkönen | Janne-Tuomas Seppänen | M. Mönkkönen | Janne-Tuomas Seppänen | J. Forsman | R. Thomson | Jukka T Forsman | Robert L Thomson | Jukka T. Forsman
[1] P. Slater,et al. Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[2] J. P. Gould. Risk, stochastic preference, and the value of information , 1974 .
[3] Sasha R. X. Dall. Defining the Concept of Public Information , 2005, Science.
[4] M. Koops. Reliability and the value of information , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[5] H. Buchanan-Smith,et al. The behavioural ecology of mixed‐species troops of callitrichine primates , 2000, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[6] Kevin N Laland,et al. Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable source when public and private information conflict , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[7] T. Valone,et al. Response: Defining the Concept of Public Information , 2005 .
[8] J. Elmberg,et al. Interspecific interactions and co-existence in dabbling ducks: observations and an experiment , 1997, Oecologia.
[9] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Responses by breeding birds to heterospecific song and mobbing call playbacks under varying predation risk , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[10] G. Uetz,et al. Foraging advantages of mixed-species association between solitary and colonial orb-weaving spiders , 1996, Oecologia.
[11] F. J. Diego-Rasilla,et al. Heterospecific call recognition and phonotaxis in the orientation behavior of the marbled newt, Triturus marmoratus , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[12] H. Pöysä. Public information and conspecific nest parasitism in goldeneyes: targeting safe nests by parasites , 2006 .
[13] M. Whiting,et al. Use of heterospecific cues by the lizard Platysaurus broadleyi for food location , 1999, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[14] Thomas R. Zentall,et al. Imitation: definitions, evidence, and mechanisms , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[15] A. Hattori. Small and large anemonefishes can coexist using the same patchy resources on a coral reef, before habitat destruction , 2002 .
[16] C. Fitzgibbon. Mixed-species grouping in Thomson's and Grant's gazelles: the antipredator benefits , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[17] E. Heymann. The relationship between body size and mixed-species troops of tamarins (Saguinus spp.). , 1997, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.
[18] Mikko Mönkkönen,et al. Numerical and behavioural responses of migrant passerines to experimental manipulation of resident tits (Parus spp.): heterospecific attraction in northern breeding bird communites? , 1990, Oecologia.
[19] M. Norconk. Introductory remarks: Ecological and behavioral correlates of polyspecific primate troops , 1990, American journal of primatology.
[20] T. Slagsvold. Habitat Selection in Birds: On the Presence of Other Bird Species with Special Regard to Turdus pilaris , 1980 .
[21] J. Biesmeijer,et al. The use of field–based social information in eusocial foragers: local enhancement among nestmates and heterospecifics in stingless bees , 2003 .
[22] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Mixed-species foraging aggregations and heterospecific attraction in boreal bird communities , 1996 .
[23] M. Groom,et al. How do frugivores track resources? Insights from spatial analyses of bird foraging in a tropical forest , 2004, Oecologia.
[24] A. Lotem,et al. The overlooked signaling component of nonsignaling behavior , 1999 .
[25] T. Valone,et al. Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[26] R. Bshary,et al. Red colobus and Diana monkeys provide mutual protection against predators , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[27] Arnon Lotem,et al. Defining the concept of public information. , 2005, Science.
[28] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Positive interactions between migrant and resident birds: testing the heterospecific attraction hypothesis , 2003, Oecologia.
[29] K. Witte,et al. Female sailfin mollies, Poecilia latipinna, remember males and copy the choice of others after 1 day , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[30] P. White,et al. The distance decay of similarity in biogeography and ecology , 1999 .
[31] Janne-Tuomas Seppänen,et al. Mechanisms and fitness effects of interspecific information use between migrant and resident birds , 2007 .
[32] I. Coolen,et al. Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[33] D. Chivers,et al. Fathead Minnows, Pimephales promelas, Learn to Recognize Chemical Alarm Cues of Introduced Brook Stickleback, Culaea inconstans , 2003, Environmental Biology of Fishes.
[34] L. Chittka,et al. Social Learning: Public Information in Insects , 2005, Current Biology.
[35] Louis Lefebvre,et al. Carib Grackles Imitate Conspecific and Zenaida Dove Tutors , 1997 .
[36] Luc-Alain Giraldeau,et al. Vicarious sampling: the use of personal and public information by starlings foraging in a simple patchy environment , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[37] A. Whiten,et al. On the Nature and Evolution of Imitation in the Animal Kingdom: Reappraisal of a Century of Research , 1992 .
[38] K. Sullivan. Information Exploitation By Downy Woodpeckers in Mixed-Species Flocks , 1984 .
[39] A. Hattori. Social and mating systems of the protandrous anemonefish Amphiprion perideraion under the influence of a larger congener , 2000 .
[40] T. Sherry,et al. Behavioral feeding specialization in Pinaroloxias inornata, the "Darwin's Finch" of Cocos Island, Costa Rica. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[41] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[42] E. D. Weinberger,et al. A theory of pragmatic information and its application to the quasi-species model of biological evolution. , 2001, Bio Systems.
[43] I. Owens,et al. MORPHOLOGICAL SHIFTS IN ISLAND‐DWELLING BIRDS: THE ROLES OF GENERALIST FORAGING AND NICHE EXPANSION , 2003, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[44] Claire Doutrelant,et al. Timing of prospecting and the value of information in a colonial breeding bird , 1996 .
[45] Robert J Fletcher,et al. Emergent properties of conspecific attraction in fragmented landscapes. , 2006, The American naturalist.
[46] C. Randler. Red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris) respond to alarm calls of Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) , 2006 .
[47] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Positive fitness consequences of interspecific interaction with a potential competitor , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[48] R. Veit,et al. Nearest neighbors as foraging cues: information transfer in a patchy environment , 2004 .
[49] David J. White,et al. Differences between the sexes in direction and duration of response to seeing a potential sex partner mate with another , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[50] C. E. SHANNON,et al. A mathematical theory of communication , 1948, MOCO.
[51] G. Tavecchia,et al. Woodpigeons nesting in association with hobby falcons: advantages and choice rules , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[52] L. Dugatkin. Mistakes and the evolution of copying , 2005 .
[53] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[54] Gerald J. Niemi,et al. Heterospecific attraction affects community structure and migrant abundances in northern breeding bird communities , 1997 .
[55] E. Goodale,et al. Testing the roles of species in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rain forest , 2005, Journal of Tropical Ecology.
[56] L. Giraldeau,et al. Social influences on foraging in vertebrates: causal mechanisms and adaptive functions , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[57] J. Mann,et al. Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[58] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Evolution of heterospecific attraction: using other species as cues in habitat selection , 2004, Evolutionary Ecology.
[59] Wladimir J Alonso,et al. Deciding where to settle: conspecific attraction and web site selection in the orb-web spider Nephilengys cruentata , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[60] I. Baldwin,et al. Priming of plant defense responses in nature by airborne signaling between Artemisia tridentata and Nicotiana attenuata , 2006, Oecologia.
[61] J. Nieh,et al. Olfactory eavesdropping by a competitively foraging stingless bee, Trigona spinipes , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[62] P. Richerson,et al. Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .
[63] W. Piper,et al. Prospecting in a solitary breeder: chick production elicits territorial intrusions in common loons , 2006 .
[64] B. Doligez,et al. Prospecting in the collared flycatcher: gathering public information for future breeding habitat selection? , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[65] J. Krebs,et al. Arms races between and within species , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[66] H. Raiffa,et al. Introduction to Statistical Decision Theory , 1996 .
[67] M. Ryan,et al. The mixed-species chorus as public information: túngara frogs eavesdrop on a heterospecific , 2007 .
[68] D. Parejo,et al. Do avian brood parasites eavesdrop on heterospecific sexual signals revealing host quality? A review of the evidence , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[69] J. Stamps. Conspecific Attraction and Aggregation in Territorial Species , 1988, The American Naturalist.
[70] B. Doligez,et al. When to use public information for breeding habitat selection? The role of environmental predictability and density dependence , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[71] A Houston,et al. The application of statistical decision theory to animal behaviour. , 1980, Journal of theoretical biology.
[72] J. Franklin,et al. Adaptive information and animal behaviour: Why motorists stop at red traffic lights , 1992 .
[73] N. R. Franks,et al. Paying for information: partial loads in central place foragers , 2006, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[74] D. Blumstein,et al. Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls , 2003 .
[75] V. V. Krishnan,et al. NONINTUITIVE CUE USE IN HABITAT SELECTION , 2005 .
[76] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Heterospecific attraction and food resources in migrants' breeding patch selection in northern boreal forest , 1998, Oecologia.
[78] E. Danchin,et al. The heterospecific habitat copying hypothesis: can competitors indicate habitat quality? , 2005 .
[79] E. Jablonka. Information: Its Interpretation, Its Inheritance, and Its Sharing , 2002, Philosophia Scientiæ.
[80] A. Whiten,et al. Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.
[81] L. Giraldeau,et al. Inadvertent social information in breeding site selection of natal dispersing birds , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[82] M. Mönkkönen,et al. Fear factor: prey habitat selection and its consequences in a predation risk landscape , 2006 .
[83] Jean Clobert,et al. Public Information and Breeding Habitat Selection in a Wild Bird Population , 2002, Science.
[84] D. Madison,et al. Behavioural responses by red-backed salamanders to conspecific and heterospecific cues , 2003 .
[85] K. Laland. Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[86] D. Stephens. Variance and the Value of Information , 1989, The American Naturalist.
[87] Robert D. Holt,et al. Temporal Autocorrelation Can Enhance the Persistence and Abundance of Metapopulations Comprised of Coupled Sinks , 2005, The American Naturalist.
[88] Sasha R. X. Dall,et al. Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[89] T. Valone,et al. Public Information: From Nosy Neighbors to Cultural Evolution , 2004, Science.