As the world turns: Short-term human spatial memory in egocentric and allocentric coordinates

We aimed to determine whether human subjects' reliance on different sources of spatial information encoded in different frames of reference (i.e., egocentric versus allocentric) affects their performance, decision time and memory capacity in a short-term spatial memory task performed in the real world. Subjects were asked to play the Memory game (a.k.a. the Concentration game) without an opponent, in four different conditions that controlled for the subjects' reliance on egocentric and/or allocentric frames of reference for the elaboration of a spatial representation of the image locations enabling maximal efficiency. We report experimental data from young adult men and women, and describe a mathematical model to estimate human short-term spatial memory capacity. We found that short-term spatial memory capacity was greatest when an egocentric spatial frame of reference enabled subjects to encode and remember the image locations. However, when egocentric information was not reliable, short-term spatial memory capacity was greater and decision time shorter when an allocentric representation of the image locations with respect to distant objects in the surrounding environment was available, as compared to when only a spatial representation encoding the relationships between the individual images, independent of the surrounding environment, was available. Our findings thus further demonstrate that changes in viewpoint produced by the movement of images placed in front of a stationary subject is not equivalent to the movement of the subject around stationary images. We discuss possible limitations of classical neuropsychological and virtual reality experiments of spatial memory, which typically restrict the sensory information normally available to human subjects in the real world.

[1]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[2]  H. Eichenbaum Hippocampus Cognitive Processes and Neural Representations that Underlie Declarative Memory , 2004, Neuron.

[3]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[4]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Orientational manoeuvres in the dark: dissociating allocentric and egocentric influences on spatial memory , 2004, Cognition.

[5]  David G. Amaral,et al.  Hippocampal Lesion Prevents Spatial Relational Learning in Adult Macaque Monkeys , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  P. Lavenex,et al.  Influence of local environmental olfactory cues on place learning in rats , 1995, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  A Berthoz,et al.  Discharge correlates of hippocampal complex spike neurons in behaving rats passively displaced on a mobile robot , 1998, Hippocampus.

[8]  S. Moffat Aging and Spatial Navigation: What Do We Know and Where Do We Go? , 2009, Neuropsychology Review.

[9]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Hippocampal function and spatial memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[10]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[11]  P. Lavenex,et al.  Spatial memory and the monkey hippocampus: Not all space is created equal , 2009, Hippocampus.

[12]  C. Gutwin,et al.  Female advantage for spatial location memory in both static and dynamic environments , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[13]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  The brain weights body‐based cues higher than vision when estimating walked distances , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Self-Motion and the Hippocampal Spatial Metric , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  P Lavenex,et al.  Integration of olfactory information in a spatial representation enabling accurate arm choice in the radial arm maze. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[16]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[17]  P. Lavenex,et al.  Olfactory Cues Potentiate Learning of Distant Visuospatial Information , 1997, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[18]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Multisensory processing in the elaboration of place and head direction responses by limbic system neurons. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[19]  Pierre Lavenex,et al.  Spatial relational learning and memory abilities do not differ between men and women in a real-world, open-field environment , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  Spatial versus nonspatial relational learning in free-ranging fox squirrels (Sciurus niger) , 1998 .

[21]  John A. King,et al.  Human hippocampus and viewpoint dependence in spatial memory , 2002, Hippocampus.

[22]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Perceiving Real-World Viewpoint Changes , 1998 .

[23]  S. Gaulin,et al.  Superior spatial memory of women: Stronger evidence for the gathering hypothesis , 1997 .

[24]  Françoise Schenk,et al.  Dissociation between Basic Components of Spatial Memory in Rats , 1995 .

[25]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Active and passive scene recognition across views , 1999, Cognition.

[26]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Interactions between idiothetic cues and external landmarks in the control of place cells and head direction cells. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  N. Burgess,et al.  The hippocampus and memory: insights from spatial processing , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.