Sixteen-year longitudinal and time lag changes in memory and cognition in older adults.

The authors describe assessment of 16-year changes in memory and intellectual abilities in a sample of 106 adults ages 30-36 and 55-81 at baseline. Results suggest that there are reliable 16-year declines in list and test recall but not in recognition after age 55. Comparisons of predictions of change from the cross-sectional baseline sample indicated that longitudinal changes were either similar to or greater than predicted for those participants over the age of 60. Despite age differences in initial scores, a comparison of 2 cohorts in a time lag analysis showed that they did not differ in performance when they were in their 70s. These results suggest that age changes in list and text recall in older adults are due to age-related declines rather than to cohort differences and that age declines in recognition are not reliable.

[1]  B. Reisberg,et al.  A Longitudinal Study of Cognitive Function in Elderly Persons with Subjective Memory Complaints , 1993, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[2]  D. Kausler Experimental psychology, cognition, and human aging , 1982 .

[3]  I. Siegler,et al.  Cross-sectional and longitudinal patterns of three Wechsler Memory Scale Subtests. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[4]  D. Kausler Learning and memory in normal aging , 1994 .

[5]  K. Schaie,et al.  Individual differences in cross-sectional and 3-year longitudinal memory performance across the adult life span. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[6]  Christopher Hertzog,et al.  Influences of cognitive slowing on age differences in intelligence. , 1989 .

[7]  K. Schaie The course of adult intellectual development. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[8]  C. Hertzog,et al.  Stability and accuracy of metamemory in adulthood and aging: a longitudinal analysis. , 1995, Psychology and aging.

[9]  R. Dixon,et al.  Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[10]  P. Costa,et al.  Adult life span changes in immediate visual memory and verbal intelligence. , 1995, Psychology and aging.

[11]  Mark L. Howe,et al.  Cognitive Development in Adulthood , 1988 .

[12]  F. Craik,et al.  Age differences in recall and recognition , 1987 .

[13]  K. Warner Schaie,et al.  Intellectual development in adulthood , 1990 .

[14]  I. Siegler,et al.  Wechsler Memory Scale Scores, selective attrition, and distance from death. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[15]  B. Reisberg,et al.  A Two-Year Longitudinal Study of Cognitive Function in Normal Aging and Alzheimer's Disease , 1993, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[16]  D. Arenberg Estimates of age changes on the Benton Visual Retention Test. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[17]  R B Wallace,et al.  Longitudinal application of cognitive function measures in a defined population of community-dwelling elders. , 1991, Annals of epidemiology.

[18]  Elizabeth M. Zelinski,et al.  Memory for Prose and Aging: A Meta-Analysis , 1988 .

[19]  K. Davis,et al.  A longitudinal study of Alzheimer's disease: measurement, rate, and predictors of cognitive deterioration. , 1994, The American journal of psychiatry.

[20]  Normal Cognitive Aging: Toward Empirical Perspectives , 1994 .

[21]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[22]  A. Heyman,et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) , 1993, Neurology.

[23]  Joy L. Taylor,et al.  Correlates of memory decline: a 4-year longitudinal study of older adults with memory complaints. , 1992 .

[24]  P. Satz,et al.  Changes in cognitive functioning associated with normal aging. , 1991, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[25]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  Theoretical Perspectives on Cognitive Aging , 1991 .