Evaluation of Neurocognition in Youth with CKD Using a Novel Computerized Neurocognitive Battery.

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES Neurocognitive problems in CKD are well documented; time-efficient methods are needed to assess neurocognition in this population. We performed the first study of the efficient 1-hour Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (CNB) in children and young adults with CKD. DESIGN, SETTING, PARTICIPANTS, & MEASUREMENTS We administered the Penn CNB cross-sectionally to individuals aged 8-25 years with stage 2-5 CKD (n=92, enrolled from three academic nephrology practices from 2011 to 2014) and matched healthy controls (n=69). We analyzed results from 12 tests in four domains: executive control, episodic memory, complex cognition, and social cognition. All tests measure accuracy and speed; we converted raw scores to age-specific z-scores on the basis of Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (n=1790) norms. We analyzed each test in a linear regression with accuracy and speed z-scores as dependent variables and with (1) CKD versus control or (2) eGFR as explanatory variables, adjusted for race, sex, and maternal education. RESULTS Patients with CKD (mean±SD eGFR, 48±25 ml/min per 1.73 m(2); mean age, 16.3±3.9 years) and controls (mean eGFR, 98±20 ml/min per 1.73 m(2); mean age, 16.0±4.0 years) were similar demographically. CKD participants had lower accuracy than controls in tests of complex cognition, with moderate to large effect sizes: -0.53 (95% confidence interval [95% CI], -0.87 to -0.19) for verbal reasoning, -0.52 (95% CI, -0.83 to -0.22) for nonverbal reasoning, and -0.64 (95% CI, -0.99 to -0.29) for spatial processing. For attention, patients with CKD had lower accuracy (effect size, -0.35 [95% CI, -0.67 to -0.03]) but faster response times (effect size, 0.44 [95% CI, 0.04 to 0.83]) than controls, perhaps reflecting greater impulsivity. Lower eGFR was associated with lower accuracy for complex cognition, facial and visual memory, and emotion identification tests. CONCLUSIONS CKD is associated with lower accuracy in tests of complex cognition, attention, memory, and emotion identification, which related to eGFR. These findings are consistent with traditional neurocognitive testing in previous studies.

[1]  Christos Davatzikos,et al.  Design and methods of the NiCK study: neurocognitive assessment and magnetic resonance imaging analysis of children and young adults with chronic kidney disease , 2015, BMC Nephrology.

[2]  Carlo Lai,et al.  Emotional self-efficacy and alexithymia may affect compliance, renal function and quality of life in kidney transplant recipients: Results from a preliminary cross-sectional study , 2015, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  Kosha Ruparel,et al.  Within-individual variability in neurocognitive performance: age- and sex-related differences in children and youths from ages 8 to 21. , 2014, Neuropsychology.

[4]  Z. Massy,et al.  Cognitive disorders and dementia in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. , 2013, Journal of the American Society of Nephrology : JASN.

[5]  N. Raz,et al.  Cognitive Function and Treatment Adherence in Older Adults With Heart Failure , 2012, Psychosomatic medicine.

[6]  M. Korkman,et al.  Neuropsychological profile of children with kidney transplants. , 2012, Nephrology, dialysis, transplantation : official publication of the European Dialysis and Transplant Association - European Renal Association.

[7]  Raquel E Gur,et al.  Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21. , 2012, Neuropsychology.

[8]  S. Shinnar,et al.  Neurocognitive functioning of children and adolescents with mild-to-moderate chronic kidney disease. , 2011, Clinical journal of the American Society of Nephrology : CJASN.

[9]  M. Haubitz,et al.  Cerebral metabolic alterations and cognitive dysfunction in chronic kidney disease. , 2011, Nephrology, dialysis, transplantation : official publication of the European Dialysis and Transplant Association - European Renal Association.

[10]  A. Murray,et al.  Cognitive impairment in peritoneal dialysis patients. , 2011, American journal of kidney diseases : the official journal of the National Kidney Foundation.

[11]  R. Shapiro,et al.  Predicting medication adherence and employment status following kidney transplant: The relative utility of traditional and everyday cognitive approaches. , 2010, Neuropsychology.

[12]  R. Gur,et al.  A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: Standardization and initial construct validation , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[13]  S. Seliger,et al.  Chronic kidney disease, creatinine and cognitive functioning. , 2009, Nephrology, dialysis, transplantation : official publication of the European Dialysis and Transplant Association - European Renal Association.

[14]  S. Hooper,et al.  Neurodevelopmental Status and Adaptive Behaviors in Preschool Children with Chronic Kidney Disease , 2009 .

[15]  P. Harris,et al.  Research electronic data capture (REDCap) - A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support , 2009, J. Biomed. Informatics.

[16]  Susan L Furth,et al.  New equations to estimate GFR in children with CKD. , 2009, Journal of the American Society of Nephrology : JASN.

[17]  S. Hooper,et al.  The central nervous system in childhood chronic kidney disease , 2007, Pediatric Nephrology.

[18]  S. Hooper,et al.  Brief report: intellectual and academic functioning in pediatric chronic kidney disease. , 2007, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[19]  S. Hooper,et al.  Clinical predictors of neurocognitive deficits in children with chronic kidney disease , 2007, Pediatric Nephrology.

[20]  S. Hooper,et al.  Memory and Executive Functions in Pediatric Chronic Kidney Disease , 2006, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[21]  Tom Greene,et al.  Using Standardized Serum Creatinine Values in the Modification of Diet in Renal Disease Study Equation for Estimating Glomerular Filtration Rate , 2006, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[22]  H. Heymans,et al.  Social consequences in adult life of end-stage renal disease in childhood. , 2005, The Journal of pediatrics.

[23]  K. Yaffe,et al.  Cognitive Impairment in Chronic Kidney Disease , 2004, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[24]  J. Blacker The impact of AIDS on adult mortality: evidence from national and regional statistics , 2004, AIDS.

[25]  P. Niaudet,et al.  LONG-TERM SOCIAL OUTCOME OF CHILDREN AFTER KIDNEY TRANSPLANTATION , 2004, Transplantation.

[26]  C. Hinkin,et al.  Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse , 2004, AIDS.

[27]  W. V. van Gorp,et al.  Neurocognitive Functioning is Associated ith Employment Status: A Quantitative Review , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[28]  A. d’Arminio Monforte,et al.  Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity , 2003, Neurology.

[29]  C. Hinkin,et al.  Medication adherence among HIV+ adults , 2002, Neurology.

[30]  R. Gur,et al.  Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: II. The Profile of Schizophrenia , 2001, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[31]  R. Gur,et al.  Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: I. Methodology and Validation in Healthy People , 2001, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[32]  A. Davey,et al.  Kidney disease and cognitive function. , 2013, Contributions to nephrology.

[33]  Lee T Copping,et al.  Sex differences in impulsivity: a meta-analysis. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  Jacqueline Dunbar-Jacob,et al.  The impact of cognitive function on medication management: three studies. , 2010, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.