Proprioceptive Stimulation Added to a Walking Self-Avatar Enhances the Illusory Perception of Walking in Static Participants

When immersed in virtual reality, users who view their body as a co-located virtual avatar that reflects their movements, generally develop a sense of embodiment whereby they perceive the virtual body to be their own. One aspect of the sense of embodiment is the feeling of agency over the avatar, i.e., the feeling that one is producing the movements of the avatar. In contexts such as physical rehabilitation, telepresence and gaming, it may be useful to induce a strong sense of agency in users who cannot produce movements or for whom it is not practical to do so. Being able to feel agency over a walking avatar without having to produce walking movements could be especially valuable. Muscle vibrations have been shown to produce the proprioceptive perception of movements, without any movement on the part of the user. The objectives of the current study were to: 1-determine if the addition of lower-limb muscle-vibrations with gait-like patterns to a walking avatar can increase the illusory perception of walking in healthy individuals who are standing still; 2-compare the effects of the complexity of the vibration patterns and of their synchronicity on the sense of agency and on the illusory perception of walking. Thirty participants viewed a walking avatar from a first-person perspective, either without muscle vibrations or with one of four different patterns of vibrations. These five conditions were presented pairwise in a two-alternative forced choice paradigm and individually presented, after which participants answered an embodiment questionnaire. The displacement of center of pressure of the participants was measured throughout the experiment. The results show that all patterns of proprioceptive stimulation increased the sense of agency to a similar degree. However, the condition in which the proprioceptive feedback was realistic and temporally aligned with the avatar’s leg movements led to significantly larger anteroposterior sway of the center of pressure. The frequency of this sway matched the cadence of the avatar’s gait. Thus, congruent and realistic proprioceptive stimulation increases the feeling of agency, the illusory perception of walking and the motor responses of the participants when viewing a walking avatar from a first-person perspective.

[1]  P. Haggard,et al.  Having a body versus moving your body: How agency structures body-ownership , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[2]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  The body schema and multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space , 2004, Cognitive Processing.

[3]  Chloé Thyrion,et al.  Predicting any arm movement feedback to induce three-dimensional illusory movements in humans. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Mel Slater,et al.  First Person Perspective of Seated Participants Over a Walking Virtual Body Leads to Illusory Agency Over the Walking , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[5]  Sharif Razzaque,et al.  Chapter 4 – Locomotion Interfaces , 2008 .

[6]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Proprioceptive signals contribute to the sense of body ownership , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  Nicolas Vuillerme,et al.  Postural sway under muscle vibration and muscle fatigue in humans , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[8]  P. Haggard,et al.  The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  M. Slater,et al.  Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Antonio Frisoli,et al.  Multisensory Feedback Can Enhance Embodiment Within an Enriched Virtual Walking Scenario , 2014, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[11]  John Quarles,et al.  Latency and avatars in Virtual Environments and the effects on gait for persons with mobility impairments , 2013, 2013 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI).

[12]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Moving a Rubber Hand that Feels Like Your Own: A Dissociation of Ownership and Agency , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[13]  Rachid Aissaoui,et al.  Modulating the Gait of a Real-Time Self-Avatar to Induce Changes in Stride Length During Treadmill Walking , 2020, 2020 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces Abstracts and Workshops (VRW).

[14]  C. Moore,et al.  Movement and the Rubber Hand Illusion , 2009, Perception.

[15]  Marc Erich Latoschik,et al.  The Impact of Implicit and Explicit Feedback on Performance and Experience during VR-Supported Motor Rehabilitation , 2021, 2021 IEEE Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR).

[16]  Mel Slater,et al.  Body ownership increases the interference between observed and executed movements , 2019, PloS one.

[17]  Neff Walker,et al.  Evaluating the importance of multi-sensory input on memory and the sense of presence in virtual environments , 1999, Proceedings IEEE Virtual Reality (Cat. No. 99CB36316).

[18]  Emily A. Keshner,et al.  The Untapped Potential of Virtual Reality in Rehabilitation of Balance and Gait in Neurological Disorders , 2021, Frontiers in Virtual Reality.

[19]  Tabitha C. Peck,et al.  Avatar Embodiment. A Standardized Questionnaire , 2021, Frontiers in Virtual Reality.

[20]  J. Roll,et al.  Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Tabitha C. Peck,et al.  Avatar Embodiment. Towards a Standardized Questionnaire , 2018, Front. Robot. AI.

[22]  Mel Slater,et al.  The Sense of Embodiment in Virtual Reality , 2012, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[23]  Kara K. Patterson,et al.  Real-Time Avatar-Based Feedback to Enhance the Symmetry of Spatiotemporal Parameters After Stroke: Instantaneous Effects of Different Avatar Views , 2020, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[24]  Mary C. Whitton,et al.  Relating Scene-Motion Thresholds to Latency Thresholds for Head-Mounted Displays , 2009, 2009 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference.

[25]  F Lacquaniti,et al.  Influence of leg muscle vibration on human walking. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  E S Grood,et al.  A joint coordinate system for the clinical description of three-dimensional motions: application to the knee. , 1983, Journal of biomechanical engineering.

[27]  Stefania Serafin,et al.  Natural Walking in Virtual Reality , 2018, Comput. Entertain..

[28]  Yasushi Ikei,et al.  Perception of Walking Self-body Avatar Enhances Virtual-walking Sensation , 2020, 2020 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces Abstracts and Workshops (VRW).

[29]  Marc Erich Latoschik,et al.  Construction of the Virtual Embodiment Questionnaire (VEQ) , 2020, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[30]  David R. Labbé,et al.  A multi-modal modified feedback self-paced BCI to control the gait of an avatar , 2021, Journal of neural engineering.

[31]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Proprioceptive Illusions Induced by Muscle Vibration: Contribution by Muscle Spindles to Perception? , 1972, Science.

[32]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[33]  G. Norman Likert scales, levels of measurement and the “laws” of statistics , 2010, Advances in health sciences education : theory and practice.

[34]  Jaron Lanier,et al.  Homuncular Flexibility in Virtual Reality , 2015, J. Comput. Mediat. Commun..

[35]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Changing bodies changes minds: owning another body affects social cognition , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  M. Slater,et al.  Measuring the Effects through Time of the Influence of Visuomotor and Visuotactile Synchronous Stimulation on a Virtual Body Ownership Illusion , 2014, Perception.

[37]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  The sense of body ownership relaxes temporal constraints for multisensory integration , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[38]  A. Berti This limb is mine but I do not want it: from anatomy to body ownership. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Justin M. Harris,et al.  If I Were You: Perceptual Illusion of Body Swapping , 2008, PloS one.

[40]  M. Slater,et al.  Embodiment in a Child-Like Talking Virtual Body Influences Object Size Perception, Self-Identification, and Subsequent Real Speaking , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[41]  Yi Yang,et al.  Investigating Implicit Gender Bias and Embodiment of White Males in Virtual Reality with Full Body Visuomotor Synchrony , 2019, CHI.

[42]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  First Person Experience of Body Transfer in Virtual Reality , 2010, PloS one.

[43]  M. Slater,et al.  Multisensory Stimulation Can Induce an Illusion of Larger Belly Size in Immersive Virtual Reality , 2011, PloS one.

[44]  James Carifio,et al.  Resolving the 50‐year debate around using and misusing Likert scales , 2008, Medical education.

[45]  Mel Slater,et al.  Body ownership causes illusory self-attribution of speaking and influences subsequent real speaking , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  Gail M. Sullivan,et al.  Analyzing and interpreting data from likert-type scales. , 2013, Journal of graduate medical education.

[47]  Rachid Aissaoui,et al.  Animated self-avatars for motor rehabilitation applications that are biomechanically accurate, low-latency and easy to use , 2016, 2016 IEEE Virtual Reality (VR).

[48]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Extending Body Space in Immersive Virtual Reality: A Very Long Arm Illusion , 2012, PloS one.

[49]  Mel Slater,et al.  How to Build an Embodiment Lab: Achieving Body Representation Illusions in Virtual Reality , 2014, Front. Robot. AI.

[50]  Janet L. Taylor,et al.  Muscle Vibration-Induced Illusions: Review of Contributing Factors, Taxonomy of Illusions and User’s Guide , 2017 .

[51]  Manos Tsakiris,et al.  Experiencing ownership over a dark-skinned body reduces implicit racial bias , 2013, Cognition.

[52]  Cyril Duclos,et al.  Complex muscle vibration patterns to induce gait-like lower-limb movements: proof of concept. , 2014, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[53]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Availability of visual and proprioceptive afferent messages and postural control in elderly adults , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  M. Whitton,et al.  Effect of latency on presence in stressful virtual environments , 2003, IEEE Virtual Reality, 2003. Proceedings..

[55]  Tomohisa Asai,et al.  Illusory body-ownership entails automatic compensative movement: for the unified representation between body and action , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  Curry Guinn,et al.  The Effects of Embodiment in Virtual Reality on Implicit Gender Bias , 2019, HCI.

[57]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Towards a Digital Body: The Virtual Arm Illusion , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[58]  Mar Gonzalez-Franco,et al.  The Self-Avatar Follower Effect in Virtual Reality , 2020, 2020 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR).

[59]  James W. Moore,et al.  What Is the Sense of Agency and Why Does it Matter? , 2016, Front. Psychol..