Cognition and Phonology

Children's speech-sound behavior has traditionally been treated as separate from other aspects of communicative behavior. In part this may have been the result of the use of the term articulation to describe this behavior. This term reflects the view that such behavior involves exclusively the acqusition of the motor ability to produce speech sounds and the perceptual ability to discriminate and perceive the acoustic consequences of these productions. However, this view overlooks a major facet of speech-sound behavior. Motor and perceptual abilities are certainly important components, along with higher level representational and organizational components as in other domains of language. The first attempts to delineate these components clearly were the proposals of generative phonologists who posited two basic levels of speech-sound behavior, phonetic and phonological (Chomsky and Halle, 1968). The phonetic level involves some representation of the actual production of speech-sound sequences. Alternately, the phonological level involves some more abstract representation of these sequences along with various types of rules which specify relationships among these representations and transform them into phonetic level realizations. This viewpoint involves a number of parallels with proposals concerning the general nature of other aspects of language as well as with nonlinguistic behaviors involving representation. Our position is that these parallels are indicative of relationships among aspects of speech-sound behavior, other aspects of language behavior, and aspects of other behaviors involving representation. The focus of this paper will be upon evidence for these relationships as well as their clinical and theoretical implications. Piaget (1962) described language as one of several outcomes of the development of the semiotic or symbolic function, the other outcomes being symbolic play, imagery and deferred imitation. More generally, this development involves the attainment of the ability to construct mental representations and perform mental operations. Related, in a somewhat tangential way, is the development of the child in the area of social cognition which early in childhood involves the disappearance of egocentricism. A renewed interest in Piaget's theory has led to a flurry of investigations of the relationships among vari-

[1]  K. Chapman,et al.  Early lexical acquisition in children with specific language impairment. , 1982, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[2]  Kathy L. Chapman,et al.  Factors Influencing Early Lexical Acquisition: Lexical Orientation and Phonological Composition. , 1981 .

[3]  L. Leonard,et al.  Early phonological behavior in normal-speaking and language disordered children: evidence for a synergistic view of linguistic disorders. , 1980, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[4]  Marlys A. Macken,et al.  Phonological Development in Children: Play and Cognition. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, No. 18. , 1980 .

[5]  L. Leonard,et al.  Some instances of word usage in the absence of comprehension , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[6]  A. Ostrowski,et al.  Effects of listener uncertainty on articulatory inconsistency. , 1979, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[7]  H. Benedict,et al.  Early lexical development: comprehension and production , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[8]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  The structure of communication in early language development , 1979 .

[9]  E. Andersen,et al.  Spontaneous Repairs: Awareness in the Process of Acquiring Language. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, No. 16. , 1979 .

[10]  L. Leonard,et al.  Some aspects of child phonology in imitative and spontaneous speech , 1978, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  L. Leonard,et al.  Language and sensorimotor development during the early period of referential speech. , 1978, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[12]  Patricia Marks Greenfield,et al.  Why Do Children Say What They Say When They Say It? An Experimental Approach to the Psychogenesis of Presupposition. , 1978 .

[13]  T. Gallagher,et al.  Conversational aspects of the speech of language-disordered children: revision behaviors. , 1978, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[14]  R. Schwartz,et al.  Sensorimotor Development and Descriptions of Child Phonology: A Preliminary View of Phonological Analysis for Stage I Speech. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, No. 13. , 1977 .

[15]  T. Gallagher,et al.  Revision behaviors in the speech of normal children developing language. , 1977, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[16]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  WORDS AND SOUNDS IN EARLY LANGUAGE ACQUISITION: ENGLISH INITIAL CONSONANTS IN THE FIRST FIFTY WORDS , 1977 .

[17]  R. Scollon,et al.  Conversations With a One Year Old: A Case Study of the Developmental Foundation of Syntax , 1976 .

[18]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  WORDS AND SOUNDS IN EARLY LANGUAGE ACQUISITION , 1975 .

[19]  E. Keenan,et al.  Conversational competence in children , 1974, Journal of Child Language.

[20]  Robert Hogan,et al.  Social Speech and Social Interaction: Egocentrism Revisited. , 1973 .

[21]  K. Nelson,et al.  Structure and strategy in learning to talk. , 1973 .

[22]  K. Lovell,et al.  A study of some aspects of the play and language of young children with delayed speech. , 1968, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[23]  L. Snyder Communicative And Cognitive Abilities And Disabilities In The Sensorimotor Period. , 1978 .

[24]  H. Beilin Constructing cognitive operations linguistically. , 1976, Advances in child development and behavior.

[25]  Jean Piaget Piaget’s Theory , 1976 .

[26]  D. Ingram Phonological Disability in Children , 1976 .

[27]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .

[28]  R. Weir,et al.  Language in the crib , 1962 .

[29]  J. Piaget Play, dreams and imitation in childhood , 1951 .