Differences in Perceptual-Motor Measures in Children with Good and Poor Handwriting

The concept that handwriting is primarily a perceptual-motor act is held by various authors (Chapman & Wedell, 1972; Furner, 1969; Sovick, 1975; Ziviani, Hayes, & Chant, 1990). However, the assumption that poor handwriters would perform less well than good handwriters on perceptual-motor tests has not yet been well researched. The purpose of this study was to examine this assumption as well as the relationship of perceptual-motor abilities to the legibility of handwriting. One hundred forty-three Chinese children in grades 3 through 5 served as subjects. Perceptual-motor tests that measured the abilities proposed to be subskills of handwriting were administered along with a handwriting test. Results showed that poor handwriters scored more poorly than good handwriters on most of the perceptual-motor tests. Regression analysis revealed that among the perceptual-motor measures visual-motor integration, as measured by the Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration, and eye-hand coordination, as measured by the Motor Accuracy Test, contributed most to the legibility of handwriting for the total group of handwriters. However, for poor handwriters, results of a stepwise regression analysis revealed that motor planning, as measured by the Finger Position Imitation Test, contributed the most to the legibility of handwriting. In contrast, for good handwriters, visual perception, as measured by the Test of Visual Perceptual Skills, contributed most to the legibility of handwriting.

[1]  J. Lindsey,et al.  Handwriting and the Classroom Experience: A Recapitulation , 1984 .

[2]  J. Ziviani,et al.  Handwriting: A Perceptual-Motor Disturbance in Children with Myelomeningocele , 1990 .

[3]  J. I. Laszlo,et al.  Kinaesthesis: Its Measurement, Training and Relationship to Motor Control , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[4]  N. Rubin,et al.  Two sides of the same coin: variations in teaching methods and failure to learn to write. , 2007, Special education: forward trends.

[5]  K. Wedell,et al.  Perceptual-Motor Abilities and Reversal Errors in Children's Handwriting , 1972 .

[6]  Hitoshi Ham Learning disabilities and brain function — A neuropsychological approach, 2nd ed, WH Gaddes. Springer-Verlag, New York·Berlin·Heidelberg·Tokyo (1985), 429pp , 1986 .

[7]  Keith E. Beery,et al.  VIM (The) : developmental test of visual-motor integration : administration, scoring, and teaching manual , 1989 .

[8]  Sally Kaminsky,et al.  Remediation of Handwriting Difficulties , 1981 .

[9]  Winnie Dunn,et al.  Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests: SIPT: Manual , 1991 .

[10]  D Colledge,et al.  Linking home and residential school. , 1982, Special education: forward trends.

[11]  Robert H. Bruininks,et al.  Bruininks - Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency , 1978 .

[12]  Phillip J. Bairstow,et al.  Handwriting: Difficulties and Possible Solutions , 1984 .

[13]  Judi Lesiak,et al.  The Relationship between Performance on the Developmental Test of Visual Perception and Handwriting Ability. , 1980 .

[14]  L. Meltzer,et al.  Developmental output failure: a study of low productivity in school-aged children. , 1981, Pediatrics.

[15]  M. Reed,et al.  Developmental Variation and Learning Disorders , 1993 .

[16]  Jean Alston,et al.  A Legibility Index: can handwriting be measured? , 1983 .

[17]  Beatrice A. Furner Recommended Instructional Procedures in a Method Emphasizing the Perceptual-Motor Nature of Learning in Handwriting. , 1969 .

[18]  Jane Taylor The Sequence and Structure of Handwriting Competence: Where are the Breakdown Points in the Mastery of Handwriting? , 1985 .