Effects of information processing demands on slow negative shift latencies and N100 amplitude in selective and divided attention

The influence of information processing demands on auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) in selective and divided attention was examined by varying the stimulation rate and target difficulty in a two-channel discrimination task. Subjects were required to detect targets in either one or both ears (channels) in a series of tones presented randomly to both ears. The amplitude of the N100 component (latency 80-140 msec) of the ERP to all stimuli in an attended channel was greater if attention was focused on that channel that if it was directed to the opposite channel. N100 amplitude was also reduced in divided attention compared to focused attention. In general, these effects were obtained only if the stimulation rate was high and/or targets were difficult to detect. However, these effects of attention modulated an underlying slow negative shift (SNS) rather than N100 directly. The onset and peak latencies of the SNS were strongly dependent on the processing demands of the task. The results show that the temporal relationship of the SNS to the N100 component is the principal factor responsible for variations in the effects of attention on the ERP with changes in information processing demands. The relationship between these ERP components and resource allocation theories of attention is discussed.

[1]  P. H. Lindsay,et al.  Quantification of shared capacity processing in auditory and visual discrimination. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[2]  N. Kroll,et al.  Autonomic responses to shock-associated words in a nonattended message: a failure to replicate. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Early selective-attention effects on the evoked potential: A critical review and reinterpretation , 1979, Biological Psychology.

[4]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. , 1978, Acta psychologica.

[5]  A. Sanford,et al.  Slow potential correlates of preparatory set. , 1974, Biological psychology.

[6]  R. S. Corteen,et al.  Autonomic responses to shock-associated words in an unattended channel. , 1972, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[7]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Selective attention and the auditory vertex potential. Effects of signal intensity and masking noise. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain , 1973, Science.

[9]  P. Michie,et al.  Different Variants of Endogenous Negative Brain Potentials in Performance Situations: A Review and Classification , 1979 .

[10]  Maryanne Martin,et al.  Reading while listening: A linear model of selective attention , 1977 .

[11]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Auditory evoked potentials during multichannel selective listening: role of pitch and localization cues. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  R. Parasuraman,et al.  Auditory evoked potentials and divided attention. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[13]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials and selective attention to acoustic and phonetic cues , 1978, Biological Psychology.

[14]  D B Lindsley,et al.  Brain wave components of the contingent negative variation in humans. , 1976, Science.

[15]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Selective attention and evoked potentials inhumans — A critical review , 1975, Biological Psychology.

[16]  E. Donchin,et al.  COGNITIVE PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY: THE ENDOGENOUS COMPONENTS OF THE ERP , 1978 .

[17]  A Treisman,et al.  Semantic processing in dichotic listening? A replication , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[18]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[19]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Visual evoked potentials and selective attention to points in space , 1977 .

[20]  J. Lewis,et al.  Semantic processing of unattended messages using dichotic listening. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  S. Hillyard,et al.  The division of attention and the human auditory evoked potential , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Electrophysiological Analysis of Human Brain Function , 1979 .

[24]  J. Deutsch,et al.  Attention: Some theoretical considerations. , 1963 .

[25]  Lawrence D. Pohlmann,et al.  Decision interaction between auditory channels , 1976 .

[26]  S. Hillyard SENSATION, PERCEPTION AND ATTENTION: ANALYSIS USING ERPs , 1978 .

[27]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Memory and data-driven control of selective attention continuous tasks. , 1979 .

[28]  John Duncan,et al.  The demonstration of capacity limitation , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Selective attention and the auditory vertex potential. I. Effects of stimulus delivery rate. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[30]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  The distribution of attention across auditory input channels: an assessment using the human evoked potential. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[31]  Peter M. Forster,et al.  Discrimination without Awareness? , 1978 .

[32]  D Robertson,et al.  Differential enhancement of early and late components of the cerebral somatosensory evoked potentials during forced‐paced cognitive tasks in man , 1977, The Journal of physiology.