A Dialogue Model of the Resolution of Inter-Individual Conflicts: Implications for Computer Based Co

As a result of empirical research into the benefits of peer interaction for learning and development, there is growing interest in designing systems to support collaborative learning. How best to do this for classroom learning is still unclear, and the aim of this thesis is to marry theory with empirical study to produce a set of guide-lines for informing the design of computer based collaborative learning. The resolution of inter-individual conflicts has long been proposed as a mechanism for learning in peer interaction and research indicates that in certain circumstances it does lead to learning. How this finding can be employed in the design of systems is problematic because researchers have typically not unpacked the notion of conflict and not explained how they are resolved. To overcome this a dialogue model of the resolution of inter-individual conflicts in joint planning is proposed. There are three key components in this model; the Task Representation, the Task Focus and the Dialogue focus. Using this model I begin to differentiate between three different types of conflict. Conflicts are caused by Task Representation Differences, Intersection Differences or Task Focus Differences . All three types of interindividual difference can be resolved with a set of discourse transactions and a set of internal resolution procedures. I propose that the resolution of all three can facilitate joint planning and subsequent individual planning. Three experiments are reported which investigate these claims. The first experiment shows that pairs performed better than individuals in a planning task and this beneficial effect carries over to subsequent individual planning. The proposed model is used to investigate this facilitative effect. A corpus of inter-individual conflicts is reported. Most of the conflicts identified in this corpus are either intersection differences or task focus differences and evidence is reported which supports the claim that their resolution can lead to learning. The second and third experiments investigate the claims made from the model about task representation differences and evidence was found to support them. The research reported in this thesis is then related to the design of software to support collaborative learning by presenting a set of guide-lines for informing their design. The work also has important implications for developmental psychology and classroom practice. i

[1]  H. Grice Utterer’s Meaning, Sentence-Meaning, and Word-Meaning , 1968 .

[2]  W. Doise,et al.  Socio-cognitive conflict and structure of individual and collective performances , 1978 .

[3]  Andrew Tolmie,et al.  Physics in the primary school: Peer interaction and the understanding of floating and sinking , 1990 .

[4]  L. S. Vygotskiĭ,et al.  Mind in society : the development of higher psychological processes , 1978 .

[5]  P. Light,et al.  «Why do you ask?» context and communication in the conservation task , 1987 .

[6]  Agnès Blaye Peer interaction in solving a binary matrix problem: possible mechanisms causing individual progress , 1990 .

[7]  L. Vygotsky Genesis of the higher mental functions. , 1991 .

[8]  W. Doise The social development of the intellect , 1984 .

[9]  Mhairi Mackenzie,et al.  Computer Support for the Collaborative Learning of Physics Concepts , 1995 .

[10]  Keith A. Coleman,et al.  On the Propositional Attitudes , 1991 .

[11]  Barbara Rogoff,et al.  Collaborative problem solving and children's planning skills. , 1989 .

[12]  D. Bearison,et al.  Socio-Cognitive Conflict and Cognitive Growth in Young Children. , 1986 .

[13]  Rachel Reichman-Adar,et al.  Extended Person-Machine Interface , 1984, Artif. Intell..

[14]  Rachel Reichman,et al.  Getting computers to talk like you and me , 1985 .

[15]  Jan Hawkins,et al.  “I'm the thinkist, you're the typist”: The Interaction of Technology and the Social Life of Classrooms , 1984 .

[16]  Barbara J. Grosz,et al.  The Representation and Use of Focus in a System for Understanding Dialogs , 1977, IJCAI.

[17]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Development of Thought: Equilibration of Cognitive Structures , 1977 .

[18]  Arthur Baskin,et al.  Learning companion systems , 1989 .

[19]  Melanie Killen,et al.  Peer interaction and the process of change in children's moral reasoning. , 1982 .

[20]  James F. Allen,et al.  A Plan Recognition Model for Subdialogues in Conversations , 1987, Cogn. Sci..

[21]  Anne-Nelly Perret-Clermont Social interaction and cognitive development in children , 1980 .

[22]  Gabriel Mugny,et al.  Individual and collective conflicts of centrations in cognitive development , 1979 .

[23]  M. Azmitia Peer Interaction and Problem Solving: When Are Two Heads Better Than One?. , 1988 .

[24]  Margaret C. Donaldson Children's Minds , 1978 .

[25]  David Trowbridge,et al.  Results from an Investigation of Groups Working at the Computer. , 1984 .

[26]  F. Murray,et al.  When two wrongs make a right: Promoting cognitive change by social conflict. , 1982 .

[27]  Courtney B. Cazden,et al.  Exploring Vygotskian Perspectives in Education: the cognitive value of peer interaction , 1985 .

[28]  Barbara J. Grosz,et al.  Focusing and Description in Natural Language Dialogues , 1979 .

[29]  Anne-Nelly Perret-Clermont,et al.  Social context effects in learning and testing , 1989 .

[30]  Rose J. Dimant,et al.  Development of formal reasoning during successive peer interactions. , 1991 .

[31]  S. Draper,et al.  The significance of dialogue in learning and observing learning , 1991 .

[32]  Dale E. Berger,et al.  Applications of Cognitive Psychology: Problem Solving, Education, and Computing , 1987 .

[33]  David W. Dalton The effects of cooperative learning strategies on achievement and attitudes during interactive video , 1990 .

[34]  Rachel Reichman,et al.  Conversational Coherency , 1978, Cogn. Sci..

[35]  Douglas H. Clements,et al.  Social-Cognitive Interactions, Motivation, and Cognitive Growth in Logo Programming and CAI Problem-Solving Environments , 1990 .

[36]  P. Light,et al.  Collaborative Interactions at the Microcomputer Keyboard , 1987 .

[37]  Earl David Sacerdoti,et al.  A Structure for Plans and Behavior , 1977 .

[38]  Andrew Tolmie,et al.  The acquisition of conceptual knowledge in science by primary school children : group interaction and the understanding of motion down an incline , 1992 .

[39]  Z. Mevarech,et al.  To Cooperate or Not to Cooperate in CAI: That Is the Question , 1987 .

[40]  Josie Taylor,et al.  Preliminary experiments with a distributed, multi-media, problem solving environ-ment , 1990 .

[41]  C. Howe,et al.  Effects of cognitive conflict, socio-cognitive conflict and imitation on children's socio-legal thinking , 1990 .

[42]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Moral Judgement of the Child , 1977 .

[43]  David Messer,et al.  A Survey of Microcomputer Use and Provision in Primary Schools , 1986 .

[44]  Martin Hughes,et al.  Girls, boys, and turtles: gender effects in young children learning with LOGO , 1990 .

[45]  Hamish Macleod,et al.  Computers and Children of Primary School Age: Issues and Questions , 1986 .

[46]  P. Bryant The role of conflict and of agreement between intellectual strategies in children's ideas about measurement , 1982 .

[47]  Richard Fikes,et al.  STRIPS: A New Approach to the Application of Theorem Proving to Problem Solving , 1971, IJCAI.

[48]  Claude Frasson,et al.  Intelligent Tutoring Systems: At the Crossroads of Artificial Intelligence and Education , 1990 .

[49]  Douglas H. Clements,et al.  Social and Cognitive Interactions in Educational Computer Environments , 1988 .

[50]  D. Laplane Thought and language. , 1992, Behavioural neurology.

[51]  J. Tudge Processes and Consequences of Peer Collaboration: A Vygotskian Analysis , 1992 .

[52]  L SidnerCandace,et al.  Attention, intentions, and the structure of discourse , 1986 .

[53]  J. Tudge When collaboration leads to regression-Some negative consequences of socio-cognitive conflict , 1989 .

[54]  J. Bruner,et al.  The role of tutoring in problem solving. , 1976, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[55]  P. Dillenbourg,et al.  A computational approach to socially distributed cognition , 1992 .

[56]  D. Bearison,et al.  Social interaction, social observation, and cognitive development in young children , 1985 .

[57]  Naomi Miyake,et al.  Constructive Interaction and the Iterative Process of Understanding , 1986, Cogn. Sci..

[58]  A. Luchins Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. , 1942 .

[59]  Paul Light,et al.  Interaction, gender, and performance on a computer-based problem solving task , 1992 .

[60]  Agnès Blaye,et al.  Collaboration as a facilitator of planning and problem solving on a computer‐based task , 1991 .

[61]  J. Piaget The Language and Thought of the Child , 1927 .

[62]  Sandra Carberry Modeling the User's Plans and Goals , 1988, Comput. Linguistics.

[63]  E. Forman,et al.  The social origins of logic: the contributions of Piaget and Vygotsky. , 1985, New directions for child development.