Phenotypic Transformation Affects Associative Learning in the Desert Locust

Summary In desert locusts, increased population densities drive phenotypic transformation from the solitarious to the gregarious phase within a generation [1–4]. Here we show that when presented with odor-food associations, the two extreme phases differ in aversive but not appetitive associative learning, with solitarious locusts showing a conditioned aversion more quickly than gregarious locusts. The acquisition of new learned aversions was blocked entirely in acutely crowded solitarious (transiens) locusts, whereas appetitive learning and prior learned associations were unaffected. These differences in aversive learning support phase-specific feeding strategies. Associative training with hyoscyamine, a plant alkaloid found in the locusts’ habitat [5, 6], elicits a phase-dependent odor preference: solitarious locusts avoid an odor associated with hyoscyamine, whereas gregarious locusts do not. Remarkably, when solitarious locusts are crowded and then reconditioned with the odor-hyoscyamine pairing as transiens, the specific blockade of aversive acquisition enables them to override their prior aversive memory with an appetitive one. Under fierce food competition, as occurs during crowding in the field, this provides a neuroecological mechanism enabling locusts to reassign an appetitive value to an odor that they learned previously to avoid.

[1]  J. Hirsch,et al.  Selection for conditionability in Drosophila melanogaster. , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[2]  E. Bernays Aversion Learning and Feeding , 1993 .

[3]  Lars Chittka,et al.  The correlation of learning speed and natural foraging success in bumble-bees , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  M. Mayer,et al.  Olfactory Conditioning of the Butterfly Agraulis vanillae (L.) (Lepidoptera, Nymphalidae) to Floral But Not Host-Plant Odors , 1999, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[5]  H. M. Smid,et al.  Differences in memory dynamics between two closely related parasitoid wasp species , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  G. B. Popov,et al.  Etude écologique des biotopes du criquet pèlerin Schistocerca gregaria (Forskal, 1775) en Afrique Nord-Occidentale : mise en évidence et description des unités territoriales écologiquement homogènes. , 1991 .

[7]  Heike Culmsee,et al.  The habitat functions of vegetation in relation to the behaviour of the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria (Forskål) (Acrididae: Orthoptera) - a study in Mauritania (West Africa) , 2002 .

[8]  C. Plowright,et al.  Discriminations of Color and Pattern on Artificial Flowers by Male and Female Bumble Bees, Bombus Impatiens (Hymenoptera: Apidae) , 2001 .

[9]  John M. Pearson,et al.  A Physiologically-Inspired Model of Numerical Classification Based on Graded Stimulus Coding , 2009, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[10]  E. Bernays,et al.  Post-ingestive feedbacks and associative learning regulate the intake of unsuitable sterols in a generalist grasshopper. , 1999, The Journal of experimental biology.

[11]  David K. Weaver,et al.  Grasshoppers and locusts , 2007 .

[12]  S. Simpson,et al.  Polyphenism in Insects , 2011, Current Biology.

[13]  Stephen J. Simpson,et al.  Parental effects on the behaviour and colouration of nymphs of the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria , 1994 .

[14]  G. Sword To be or not to be a locust? A comparative analysis of behavioral phase change in nymphs of Schistocerca americana and S. gregaria. , 2003, Journal of insect physiology.

[15]  Leslie A. Real,et al.  Learning foraging tasks by bees: a comparison between social and solitary species , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  S. J. Simpson,et al.  Surviving the change to warning colouration: density-dependent polyphenism suggests a route for the evolution of aposematism , 2005, CHEMOECOLOGY.

[17]  L. Vet,et al.  Comparison of learning in related generalist and specialist eucoilid parasitoids , 1992 .

[18]  Ken Sasaki,et al.  Substantial changes in central nervous system neurotransmitters and neuromodulators accompany phase change in the locust , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[19]  W. Gronenberg,et al.  Africanized honeybees are slower learners than their European counterparts , 2009, Naturwissenschaften.

[20]  Stephen J. Simpson,et al.  Compensation by locusts for changes in dietary nutrients: behavioural mechanisms , 1985 .

[21]  T. C. Ings,et al.  Unterschiede im Lernverhalten zwischen Kolonien einer freilebenden Britischen Hummelpopulation (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombus terrestris audax) , 2006 .

[22]  B. P. Uvarov,et al.  A revision of the genus Locusta, L. (= Pachytylus, Fieb.), with a new theory as to the periodicity and migrations of locusts , 1921 .

[23]  R. Dadd The nutritional requirements of locusts—I Development of synthetic diets and lipid requirements , 1960 .

[24]  Stephen J. Simpson,et al.  A behavioural analysis of phase change in the desert locust , 1999 .

[25]  E. Despland,et al.  Food choices of solitarious and gregarious locusts reflect cryptic and aposematic antipredator strategies , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  L. Chittka,et al.  A population comparison of the strength and persistence of innate colour preference and learning speed in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris , 2009, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[27]  Elizabeth A. Tibbetts,et al.  Specialized Face Learning Is Associated with Individual Recognition in Paper Wasps , 2011, Science.

[28]  Matthew Collett,et al.  Small‐scale processes in desert locust swarm formation: how vegetation patterns influence gregarization , 2000 .

[29]  Comparison of associative learning of host-related plant volatiles in two parasitoids with different degrees of host specificity, Cotesia marginiventris and Microplitis croceipes , 2012, Chemoecology.

[30]  I. Kandori,et al.  Interspecific and intersexual learning rate differences in four butterfly species , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[31]  S. Mukherjee,et al.  Absence of Food Aversion Learning in the Polyphagous Noctuid, Spodoptera litura (F.) Following Intoxication by Deleterious Chemicals , 2005, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[32]  T. Bukovinszky,et al.  Species-specific acquisition and consolidation of long-term memory in parasitic wasps , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  C. Brandes Genetic differences in learning behavior in honeybees (Apis mellifera capensis) , 1991, Behavior genetics.

[34]  D. Potter,et al.  Absence of food‐aversion learning by a polyphagous scarab, Popillia japonica, following intoxication by geranium, Pelargonium × hortorum , 1999 .

[35]  Gilles Laurent,et al.  Olfactory network dynamics and the coding of multidimensional signals , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  Stephen J. Simpson,et al.  Locust Phase Polyphenism: An Update , 2009 .

[37]  Jeremy E Niven,et al.  A long-latency aversive learning mechanism enables locusts to avoid odours associated with the consequences of ingesting toxic food , 2012, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[38]  S. J. Simpson,et al.  Density–dependent aposematism in the desert locust , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  B. P. Uvarov,et al.  Phases of Locusts and their Interrelations , 1929 .

[40]  Jeffrey A. Riffell,et al.  The neurobiology of insect olfaction: Sensory processing in a comparative context , 2011, Progress in Neurobiology.

[41]  K. Hartfelder,et al.  Odor discrimination in classical conditioning of proboscis extension in two stingless bee species in comparison to Africanized honeybees , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[42]  A. Mercer,et al.  Queen Pheromone Blocks Aversive Learning in Young Worker Bees , 2007, Science.

[43]  F. James Rohlf,et al.  Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .

[44]  Jeremy E. Niven,et al.  Associative olfactory learning in the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[45]  S. Simpson,et al.  Density‐dependent accumulation of phase characteristics in a natural population of the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria , 2003 .

[46]  Sokal Rr,et al.  Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research 2nd edition. , 1981 .

[47]  Stephen J. Simpson,et al.  Analysis of phase-related changes in behaviour of desert locust nymphs , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  A. Moczek,et al.  Phenotypic plasticity and diversity in insects , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.