The influence of sad mood on cognition.

Neuroimaging has identified an overlapping network of brain regions whose activity is modulated by mood and cognition. Studies of depressed individuals have shown changes in perception, attention, memory, and executive functions. This suggests that mood has a pervasive effect on cognition. Direct evidence of the effect of sad mood on cognition is surprisingly limited, however. Published studies have generally addressed a single cognitive ability per study because the fleeting nature of laboratory-induced mood precludes extended testing, and robust findings are limited to mood effects on memory for emotional stimuli. In this study, sad mood was induced and prolonged, enabling the effects of mood to be assessed for an array of abilities, including those that share neural substrates with sad mood and those affected by depression. Sad mood affected memory for emotional words and facial emotion recognition, but not the other processes measured, with a significant nonuniformity of effect over tasks. These results are consistent with circumscribed effects of sad mood on certain emotion-related cognitive processes, but not on cognition more generally.

[1]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Attentional bias in emotional disorders. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[2]  E. Walker,et al.  Facial discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. , 1986, Archives of general psychiatry.

[3]  J. Persons,et al.  Dysfunctional attitudes are mood-state dependent. , 1988, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[4]  E. L. Cooley,et al.  Discrimination of facial expressions of emotion by depressed subjects. , 1989, Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs.

[5]  Henry C. Ellis,et al.  A convenient self-referencing mood induction procedure , 1991 .

[6]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: artefact, processing priorities or attentional search? , 1991, Behaviour research and therapy.

[7]  J. Williams Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders , 1991 .

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The anatomy of melancholia – focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression , 1992, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  H. Kraemer,et al.  Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression , 1992, Psychiatry Research.

[10]  M. Raichle,et al.  A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  Dc Washington Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .

[12]  A. Bouhuys,et al.  Induction of depressed and elated mood by music influences the perception of facial emotional expressions in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of affective disorders.

[13]  E. Mikhailova,et al.  Abnormal recognition of facial expression of emotions in depressed patients with major depression disorder and schizotypal personality disorder , 1996, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  D. Williamson,et al.  Evidence for attention to threatening stimuli in depression. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[16]  Z. Segal,et al.  Effects of self-generated sad mood on regional cerebral activity: a PET study in normal subjects. , 1996, Depression.

[17]  M. Oaksford,et al.  Mood, reasoning, and central executive processes. , 1996 .

[18]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[19]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Working Memory: A View from Neuroimaging , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[21]  R. Lane,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[22]  A. Bouhuys,et al.  Relationship between perception of facial emotions and anxiety in clinical depression: does anxiety-related perception predict persistence of depression? , 1997, Journal of affective disorders.

[23]  D. Wechsler Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale , 2021, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[24]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[25]  I. Gotlib,et al.  The Effects of Induced and Naturally Occurring Dysphoric Mood on Biases in Self-evaluation and Memory , 1997 .

[26]  J Jonides,et al.  Spatial, but not object, delayed response is impaired in early Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[27]  Mood induction with older adults: a tool for investigating effects of depressed mood. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Reproducibility of fMRI Results across Four Institutions Using a Spatial Working Memory Task , 1998, NeuroImage.

[29]  H. Ellis,et al.  Cognitive activity and physiological arousal: Processes that mediate mood-congruent memory , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[30]  T. Glauser,et al.  Effects of experimentally-induced emotional states on frontal lobe cognitive task performance , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  W. Drevets Prefrontal Cortical‐Amygdalar Metabolism in Major Depression , 1999, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[32]  A. Bouhuys,et al.  Depressed patients' perceptions of facial emotions in depressed and remitted states are associated with relapse: a longitudinal study. , 1999, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[33]  A. Anderson,et al.  An fMRI study of stroop word-color interference: evidence for cingulate subregions subserving multiple distributed attentional systems , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[34]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[35]  I. Hickie,et al.  Cognitive function in depression: a distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? , 1999, Psychological Medicine.

[36]  M Gallardo Pérez,et al.  Attentional biases and vulnerability to depression. , 1999, The Spanish journal of psychology.

[37]  L. Parsons,et al.  Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[38]  D. Levine,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. , 1999, Psychological review.

[39]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[41]  P. Fox,et al.  Differential limbic–cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[42]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  A. Leuchter,et al.  Executive dysfunction predicts nonresponse to fluoxetine in major depression. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[44]  S. Houle,et al.  Changes in regional brain glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography after paroxetine treatment of major depression. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[45]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Event‐related fMRI study of response inhibition , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[46]  J. J. Ryan,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III , 2001 .

[47]  K. Fiedler,et al.  Is mood congruency an effect of genuine memory or response bias , 2001 .

[48]  J. Forgas Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition , 2001 .

[49]  S. Moritz,et al.  Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia , 2002 .

[50]  P. Näätänen,et al.  Neuroanatomical substrata of amusement and sadness: a PET activation study using film stimuli , 2002 .

[51]  R. Davidson,et al.  Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[52]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Emotion Activation Studies in PET and fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[53]  J. Haxby,et al.  Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[54]  L. Phillips,et al.  Positive mood and executive function: evidence from stroop and fluency tasks. , 2002, Emotion.

[55]  B. Mensour,et al.  Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[56]  W. Drevets Neuroimaging Abnormalities in the Amygdala in Mood Disorders , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Fanny Eugène,et al.  The impact of individual differences on the neural circuitry underlying sadness , 2003, NeuroImage.

[58]  A. Lawrence,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[59]  S. Rauch,et al.  Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[60]  S. Rauch,et al.  Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[61]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Stroop Interference following Mood Induction: Emotionality, Mood Congruence, and Concern Relevance , 2004, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[62]  T. Dalgleish The emotional brain , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[63]  J. D Steele,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and emotional function in the prefrontal cortex: a stereotactic meta-analysis , 2004, NeuroImage.

[64]  B. Dubois,et al.  Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depression. , 2004, Journal of psychiatric research.

[65]  S. Paradiso,et al.  Cognitive and neurological impairment in mood disorders. , 2004, The Psychiatric clinics of North America.

[66]  D. Algom,et al.  A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[67]  Masato Fukuda,et al.  Executive and prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: a review of neuropsychological and imaging evidence , 2004, Neuroscience Research.

[68]  Dinkar Sharma,et al.  Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[69]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Cognitive Vulnerability for Depression: Deployment of Attention as a Function of History of Depression and Current Mood State , 2000, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[70]  J. Hietanen,et al.  Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces , 2004, Psychiatry Research.

[71]  Scott A Langenecker,et al.  Face Emotion Perception and Executive Functioning Deficits in Depression , 2005, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[72]  Seung-Lark Lim,et al.  Cognitive processing of emotional information in depression, panic, and somatoform disorder. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[73]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Contributions of the Amygdala to Emotion Processing: From Animal Models to Human Behavior , 2005, Neuron.