Place recognition and heading retrieval are mediated by dissociable cognitive systems in mice

Significance The ability to recover one’s bearings when lost is critical for successful navigation. To accomplish this feat, a navigator must identify its current location (place recognition), and it must also recover its facing direction (heading retrieval). Using a novel behavioral paradigm, we demonstrate that mice use one set of cues to determine their location and then ignore these same cues when determining their heading, although the cues are informative in both cases. These results suggest that place recognition and heading retrieval are mediated by different processing systems that operate in partial independence of each other. This finding has important implications for understanding the cognitive architecture underlying spatial navigation. A lost navigator must identify its current location and recover its facing direction to restore its bearings. We tested the idea that these two tasks—place recognition and heading retrieval—might be mediated by distinct cognitive systems in mice. Previous work has shown that numerous species, including young children and rodents, use the geometric shape of local space to regain their sense of direction after disorientation, often ignoring nongeometric cues even when they are informative. Notably, these experiments have almost always been performed in single-chamber environments in which there is no ambiguity about place identity. We examined the navigational behavior of mice in a two-chamber paradigm in which animals had to both recognize the chamber in which they were located (place recognition) and recover their facing direction within that chamber (heading retrieval). In two experiments, we found that mice used nongeometric features for place recognition, but simultaneously failed to use these same features for heading retrieval, instead relying exclusively on spatial geometry. These results suggest the existence of separate systems for place recognition and heading retrieval in mice that are differentially sensitive to geometric and nongeometric cues. We speculate that a similar cognitive architecture may underlie human navigational behavior.

[1]  Magdalena G. Wutte,et al.  Modality-Independent Coding of Spatial Layout in the Human Brain , 2011, Current Biology.

[2]  E. Maguire,et al.  What does the retrosplenial cortex do? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  Jonathan S. Cant,et al.  Object Ensemble Processing in Human Anterior-Medial Ventral Visual Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[5]  M. Moser,et al.  Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  Sang Ah Lee,et al.  Young Children Reorient by Computing Layout Geometry, Not by Matching Images of the Environment , 2010 .

[7]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[8]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[9]  W. Gerstner,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Insights from a rodent navigation model. , 2009, Psychological review.

[10]  Alexandra D. Twyman,et al.  Of mice (Mus musculus) and toddlers (Homo sapiens): evidence for species-general spatial reorientation. , 2009, Journal of comparative psychology.

[11]  Russell A. Epstein Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  H. Jeffreys A Treatise on Probability , 1922, Nature.

[13]  Laurie L Bloomfield,et al.  Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry , 2005, Biology Letters.

[14]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Abstract Representations of Location and Facing Direction in the Human Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  P. Wilson,et al.  Blocking of spatial learning between enclosure geometry and a local landmark. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[17]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[18]  J. Pearce,et al.  Failure of a landmark to restrict spatial learning based on the shape of the environment , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[19]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  An Area within Human Ventral Cortex Sensitive to “Building” Stimuli Evidence and Implications , 1998, Neuron.

[20]  Ken Cheng,et al.  25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[21]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape , 1988 .

[22]  Marko Nardini,et al.  ' s personal copy A viewpoint-independent process for spatial reorientation , 2009 .

[23]  Peter M. Jones,et al.  Potentiation, overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on the shape of the environment. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[24]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[25]  Guillem R. Esber,et al.  Absence of overshadowing between a landmark and geometric cues in a distinctively shaped environment: a test of Miller and Shettleworth (2007). , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[26]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[27]  J. Taube The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[28]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Medial Parietal Cortex Encodes Perceived Heading Direction in Humans , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  J. Pearce,et al.  Absence of Overshadowing and Blocking between Landmarks and the Geometric Cues Provided by the Shape of a Test Arena , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[31]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Geometry, features, and panoramic views: ants in rectangular arenas. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[32]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[33]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Anchoring the neural compass: Coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Pearce,et al.  Spatial learning based on the shape of the environment is influenced by properties of the objects forming the shape. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[35]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  1 Explaining the Development of Spatial Reorientation : Modularity-Plus-Language Versus the Emergence of Adaptive Combination , 2007 .

[36]  Seralynne D Vann,et al.  Do rats with retrosplenial cortex lesions lack direction? , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  Yutaka Kosaki,et al.  Overshadowing of geometry learning by discrete landmarks in the water maze: effects of relative salience and relative validity of competing cues. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[38]  M. Eacott,et al.  Dissociable effects of lesions to the perirhinal cortex and the postrhinal cortex on memory for context and objects in rats. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[39]  Z. Dienes Bayesian Versus Orthodox Statistics: Which Side Are You On? , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[40]  Sang Ah Lee,et al.  Chicks, like children, spontaneously reorient by three-dimensional environmental geometry, not by image matching , 2012, Biology Letters.

[41]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  J. Taube Head direction cells and the neurophysiological basis for a sense of direction , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[43]  J. Pearce,et al.  Spatial Learning Based on Boundaries in Rats Is Hippocampus-Dependent and Prone to Overshadowing , 2010, Behavioral neuroscience.

[44]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  J. Pearce,et al.  A landmark blocks searching for a hidden platform in an environment with a distinctive shape after extended pretraining , 2009, Learning & behavior.

[46]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Importance of Proving the Null , 2022 .

[47]  J. Fodor,et al.  The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology , 1984 .

[48]  D. Amaral,et al.  Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices of the rat: A review of the neuroanatomical literature and comparison with findings from the monkey brain , 1995, Hippocampus.

[49]  Robin Hayman,et al.  Geometric Cues Influence Head Direction Cells Only Weakly in Nondisoriented Rats , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[51]  Allen Cheung,et al.  The information content of panoramic images I: The rotational errors and the similarity of views in rectangular experimental arenas. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[52]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Explaining the Development of Spatial Reorientation , 2007 .

[53]  M. Moser,et al.  Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex , 2008, Science.

[54]  Daniel D. Dilks,et al.  Mirror-Image Sensitivity and Invariance in Object and Scene Processing Pathways , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Lesions of the perirhinal cortex do not impair integration of visual and geometric information in rats. , 2010, Behavioral neuroscience.

[56]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  Real-World Scene Representations in High-Level Visual Cortex: It's the Spaces More Than the Places , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Soojin Park,et al.  Disentangling Scene Content from Spatial Boundary: Complementary Roles for the Parahippocampal Place Area and Lateral Occipital Complex in Representing Real-World Scenes , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi & Polli (2004). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.