Influence of consonantal context on the pronunciation of vowels: A comparison of human readers and computational models

In two experiments, we found that college students' pronunciations of vowels in nonwords are influenced both by preceding and following consonants. The predominance of rimes in previous studies of reading does not appear to arise because readers are unable to pick up associations that cross the onset-rime boundary, but rather because English has relatively few such associations. Comparisons between people's vowel pronunciations and those produced by various computational models of reading showed that no model provided a good account of human performance on nonwords for which the vowel shows contextual conditioning. Possible directions for improved models are suggested.

[1]  David A. Balota,et al.  Bringing Computational Models of Word Naming Down to the Item Level , 1997 .

[2]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Relationships between sounds and letters in English monosyllables , 2001 .

[3]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Generalization with Componential Attractors: Word and Nonword Reading in an Attractor Network , 1993 .

[4]  G. Stone,et al.  Perception Is a Two-Way Street: Feedforward and Feedback Phonology in Visual Word Recognition ☆ , 1997 .

[5]  P. David Pearson,et al.  Influence of Type-Token Frequencies and Final Consonants on Adults' Internalization of Vowel Digraphs. , 1980 .

[6]  Beatrice Santorini,et al.  Building a Large Annotated Corpus of English: The Penn Treebank , 1993, CL.

[7]  Use of Spelling-to-Sound Rules in Reading , 1976 .

[8]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  On the complexities of measuring naming. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  C M Herdman,et al.  Imageability effects in word naming: an individual differences analysis. , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[10]  David R. Shanks,et al.  The Psychology of Associative Learning , 1995 .

[11]  R. Venezky The Structure of English Orthography , 1965 .

[12]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Surface dyslexia. , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[14]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Spelling-sound effects in reading: Time-course and decision criteria , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[15]  James L. McClelland,et al.  CONNECTIONS AND DISCONNECTIONS: A CONNECTIONIST ACCOUNT OF SURFACE DYSLEXIA , 1996 .

[16]  M Coltheart,et al.  Reading aloud begins when the computation of phonology is complete. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  D. Norris A quantitative multiple-levels model of reading aloud. , 1994 .

[18]  G. C. Orden A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading , 1987 .

[19]  R. Glushko The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .

[20]  Debra Jared,et al.  Spelling-Sound Consistency Affects the Naming of High-Frequency Words , 1997 .

[21]  R. Treiman,et al.  Context sensitivity in the spelling of English vowels , 2002 .

[22]  Sally Andrews,et al.  Rule and analogy mechanisms in reading nonwords: Hough dou peapel rede gnew wirds? , 1998 .

[23]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. , 1987 .

[24]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[25]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[26]  Richard L. Venezky,et al.  Models for Predicting How Adults Pronounce Vowel Digraph Spellings in Unfamiliar Words. , 1975 .

[27]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[28]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Nonword pronunciation and models of word recognition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Initial phoneme versus whole-word criterion to initiate pronunciation: Evidence based on response latency and initial phoneme duration. , 1998 .

[30]  M. Humphreys,et al.  Delayed Priming of the Pronunciation of Inconsistent Words and Pseudowords , 1993 .

[31]  R. Peereman,et al.  Is perception a two-way street ?: The case of feedback consistency in visual word recognition , 1998 .

[32]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[33]  R. Berndt,et al.  Neural Modeling of Brain and Cognitive Disorders , 1996 .

[34]  G. V. van Orden A ROWS is a ROSE: spelling, sound, and reading. , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[35]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Phonetic Biases in Voice Key Response Time Measurements , 2002 .

[36]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on nonword reading , 1998 .

[37]  R. Treiman,et al.  Units in reading and spelling , 1988 .

[38]  D. A. Allport,et al.  Language perception and production : relationships between listening, speaking, reading and writing , 1987 .

[39]  M Coltheart,et al.  Serial processing in reading aloud: reply to Zorzi (2000). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Patricia A. McMullen,et al.  Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia , 1999 .

[41]  Ken N. Seergobin,et al.  On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .

[42]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  D. Jared Spelling-Sound Consistency and Regularity Effects in Word Naming , 2002 .