Indexing the approximate number system.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction , 2007, Nature.
[2] Daniel Ansari,et al. The role of the left intraparietal sulcus in the relationship between symbolic number processing and children's arithmetic competence , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[3] Justin Halberda,et al. Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[4] Marco Zorzi,et al. A Computational Model of Number Comparison , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
[5] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Nonsymbolic Arithmetic in Adults and Young Children , 2003 .
[6] E. Spelke,et al. Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .
[7] R. Church,et al. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[8] Bert De Smedt,et al. Numerical Magnitude Representations and Individual Differences in Children's Arithmetic Strategy Use , 2012 .
[9] Sian L. Beilock,et al. Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence , 2011, Cognition.
[10] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic quantity processing in 5-year olds and adults , 2008, Cognitive Processing.
[11] Daniel Ansari,et al. A Two-Minute Paper-and-Pencil Test of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing Explains Variability in Primary School Children's Arithmetic Competence , 2013, PloS one.
[12] B. Reynvoet,et al. Predictors for Mathematics Achievement? Evidence From a Longitudinal Study , 2012 .
[13] Bert Reynvoet,et al. Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[14] Anthony J Bishara,et al. Testing the significance of a correlation with nonnormal data: comparison of Pearson, Spearman, transformation, and resampling approaches. , 2012, Psychological methods.
[15] Justin Halberda,et al. Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.
[16] Janosch Linkersdörfer,et al. Symbolic and non-symbolic distance effects in children and their connection with arithmetic skills , 2011, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[17] Matthew Inglis,et al. Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[18] S. Dehaene,et al. Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.
[19] Bert Reynvoet,et al. Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli , 2011, Behavior research methods.
[20] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. , 2011, Developmental science.
[21] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Nonsymbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[22] Bert Reynvoet,et al. The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[23] Matthew Inglis,et al. Measuring the Approximate Number System , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[24] Xinlin Zhou,et al. Cognitive correlates of performance in advanced mathematics. , 2012, The British journal of educational psychology.
[25] Alain Content,et al. Judgement of discrete and continuous quantity in adults: Number counts! , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[26] Rochel Gelman,et al. Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] Wim Fias,et al. Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] Bert Reynvoet,et al. Continuous visual properties explain neural responses to nonsymbolic number. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[29] C. Gilmore,et al. Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[30] Justin Halberda,et al. Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.
[31] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[32] Andrea Facoetti,et al. Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.
[33] C. Kowalski. On the Effects of Non‐Normality on the Distribution of the Sample Product‐Moment Correlation Coefficient , 1972 .
[34] Jason W. Osborne,et al. Best Practices in Data Transformation: The Overlooked Effect of Minimum Values , 2008 .
[35] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool years. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[36] Neil Marlow,et al. Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement , 2013, PloS one.
[37] L. Aiken. Psychological testing and assessment , 1976 .
[38] L. Feigenson,et al. Preschoolers' Precision of the Approximate Number System Predicts Later School Mathematics Performance , 2011, PloS one.
[39] Daniel Ansari,et al. Challenging the reliability and validity of cognitive measures: the case of the numerical distance effect. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[40] Matthew Inglis,et al. Sampling from the mental number line: How are approximate number system representations formed? , 2013, Cognition.
[41] Daniel Ansari,et al. Using eye tracking to study numerical cognition: the case of the ratio effect , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] Lieven Verschaffel,et al. Do numerical magnitude comparison skills predict individual differences in mathematical achievement , 2009 .
[43] Evelyn H. Kroesbergen,et al. Early Numerical Development and the Role of Non-Symbolic and Symbolic Skills. , 2013 .
[44] Justin Halberda,et al. Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[45] Daniel Ansari,et al. Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[46] Daniel Ansari,et al. Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[47] Jacqueline Leybaert,et al. Does math education modify the approximate number system? A comparison of schooled and unschooled adults , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.
[48] Rochel Gelman,et al. Sometimes area counts more than number , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[49] Manuela Piazza,et al. Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[50] Nicole M. McNeil,et al. ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. , 2013, Developmental science.
[51] Justin Halberda,et al. Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.
[52] Silke M. Göbel,et al. Impact of High Mathematics Education on the Number Sense , 2012, PloS one.
[53] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.