Generalization via superposition: combined effects of mixed reference frame representations for explicit and implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task.

Studies on generalization of learned visuomotor perturbations have generally focused on whether learning is coded in extrinsic or intrinsic reference frames. This dichotomy, however, is challenged by recent findings showing that learning is represented in a mixed reference frame. Overlooked in this framework is how learning appears to consist of multiple processes, such as explicit reaiming and implicit motor adaptation. Therefore, the proposed mixed representation may simply reflect the superposition of explicit and implicit generalization functions, each represented in different reference frames. Here we characterized the individual generalization functions of explicit and implicit learning in relative isolation to determine whether their combination could predict the overall generalization function when both processes are in operation. We modified the form of feedback in a visuomotor rotation task in an attempt to isolate explicit and implicit learning and tested generalization across new limb postures to dissociate the extrinsic/intrinsic representations. We found that the amplitude of explicit generalization was reduced with postural change and was only marginally shifted, resembling an extrinsic representation. In contrast, implicit generalization maintained its amplitude but was significantly shifted, resembling a mixed representation. A linear combination of individual explicit and implicit generalization functions accounted for nearly 85% of the variance associated with the generalization function in a typical visuomotor rotation task, where both processes are in operation. This suggests that each form of learning results from a mixed representation with distinct extrinsic and intrinsic contributions and the combination of these features shapes the generalization pattern observed at novel limb postures. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Generalization following learning in visuomotor adaptation tasks can reflect how the brain represents what it learns. In this study, we isolated explicit and implicit forms of learning and showed that they are derived from a mixed reference frame representation with distinct extrinsic and intrinsic contributions. Furthermore, we showed that the overall generalization pattern at novel workspaces is due to the superposition of independent generalization effects developed by explicit and implicit learning processes.

[1]  Eugene Poh,et al.  Distinct coordinate systems for adaptations of movement direction and extent. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The effect of contextual cues on the encoding of motor memories , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Krista M Bond,et al.  Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Maurice A. Smith,et al.  Motor Memory Is Encoded as a Gain-Field Combination of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Action Representations , 2012, Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  R. H. Franke,et al.  The Hawthorne experiments: First statistical interpretation. , 1978 .

[7]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Feedback-dependent generalization. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[9]  Richard A Andersen,et al.  Integration of target and hand position signals in the posterior parietal cortex: effects of workspace and hand vision. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.

[12]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Adaptation to visuomotor rotations in younger and older adults. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[13]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Andrew P Yonelinas,et al.  The process-dissociation approach two decades later: Convergence, boundary conditions, and new directions , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[15]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Generalization of Motor Learning Depends on the History of Prior Action , 2006, PLoS biology.

[16]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[17]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  John W Krakauer,et al.  Adaptation to visuomotor rotation through interaction between posterior parietal and motor cortical areas. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Implications of plan-based generalization in sensorimotor adaptation. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Sensory integration for reaching: models of optimality in the context of behavior and the underlying neural circuits. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[21]  Claude Ghez,et al.  Separate adaptive mechanisms for controlling trajectory and final position in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Plan-based generalization shapes local implicit adaptation to opposing visuomotor transformations , 2018, bioRxiv.

[23]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Adaptation to a visuomotor shift depends on the starting posture. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Learning not to generalize: modular adaptation of visuomotor gain. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[26]  R. Shepard,et al.  Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. , 1987, Science.

[27]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Howard G. Wu,et al.  The Generalization of Visuomotor Learning to Untrained Movements and Movement Sequences Based on Movement Vector and Goal Location Remapping , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  R. H. Franke,et al.  Worker Productivity at Hawthorne , 1980 .

[30]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Ilana Nisky,et al.  Learning and generalization in an isometric visuomotor task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  E. Brenner,et al.  Illusions as a tool to study the coding of pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  R. Shadmehr Generalization as a behavioral window to the neural mechanisms of learning internal models. , 2004, Human movement science.

[34]  R. Ivry,et al.  Delayed feedback during sensorimotor learning selectively disrupts adaptation but not strategy use. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Sinusoidal error perturbation reveals multiple coordinate systems for sensorymotor adaptation , 2016, Vision Research.

[37]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Preparatory activity in motor cortex reflects learning of local visuomotor skills , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Aymar de Rugy,et al.  The synergistic organization of muscle recruitment constrains visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Konrad P. Körding,et al.  Estimating the Relevance of World Disturbances to Explain Savings, Interference and Long-Term Motor Adaptation Effects , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[42]  Olivier White,et al.  Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Visuomotor Learning Generalizes Around the Intended Movement123 , 2016, eNeuro.

[44]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Generalization of implicit and explicit adjustments to visuomotor rotations across the workspace in younger and older adults. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  Jordan A. Taylor,et al.  Effect of coordinate frame compatibility on the transfer of implicit and explicit learning across limbs. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  E Bizzi,et al.  Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  E. Brenner,et al.  Endpoints of arm movements to visual targets , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Gone in 0.6 Seconds: The Encoding of Motor Memories Depends on Recent Sensorimotor States , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Dissociable cognitive strategies for sensorimotor learning , 2019, Nature Communications.

[50]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Rotations Remaps Movement Vectors, Not Final Positions , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Motor learning reveals the existence of multiple codes for movement planning. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Generalization in vision and motor control , 2004, Nature.

[53]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Feedback delay attenuates implicit but facilitates explicit adjustments to a visuomotor rotation , 2017, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[54]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Characteristics of Implicit Sensorimotor Adaptation Revealed by Task-irrelevant Clamped Feedback , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.