The Source of the Symbolic Numerical Distance and Size Effects
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jonathan W. Peirce,et al. PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[2] J. Hummel,et al. Why spatial-numeric associations aren't evidence for a mental number line , 2008 .
[3] U. Cress,et al. How number-space relationships are assessed before formal schooling: A taxonomy proposal , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[4] Philip L. Smith,et al. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[5] S. Dehaene,et al. Single-trial classification of parallel pre-attentive and serial attentive processes using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[6] Philippe Pinel,et al. Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.
[7] R. Ratcliff,et al. Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[8] E. Spelke,et al. Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .
[9] Michaël A. Stevens,et al. A model of exact small-number representation , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] M. Just,et al. Decoding the representation of numerical values from brain activation patterns , 2013, Human brain mapping.
[11] Tali Leibovich,et al. The symbol-grounding problem in numerical cognition: A review of theory, evidence, and outstanding questions. , 2016, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[12] A. Idrissi,et al. On the Mental Representation of , 2008 .
[13] J. Jonides,et al. Working memory for order and the parietal cortex: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2006, Neuroscience.
[14] M. Shadlen,et al. The effect of stimulus strength on the speed and accuracy of a perceptual decision. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[15] E. R. Grossman. The Measurement of Discriminability , 1955 .
[16] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[17] C Leth-Steensen,et al. A model of response time effects in symbolic comparison. , 2000, Psychological review.
[18] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[19] Tom Verguts,et al. A delta-rule model of numerical and non-numerical order processing. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. The Representation of Negative Numbers: Exploring the Effects of Mode of Processing and Notation , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[21] Daniel Ansari,et al. Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[22] S. Dehaene. Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .
[23] Bert Reynvoet,et al. Children's representation of symbolic magnitude: the development of the priming distance effect. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[24] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[25] Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al. Beyond the number domain , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[26] Gordon D. Logan,et al. What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. , 2005 .
[27] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: A case study of mathematics , 2004 .
[28] W. Levelt,et al. Semantic distance effects on object and action naming , 2002, Cognition.
[29] J. Bulthé,et al. Format-dependent representations of symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as revealed by multi-voxel pattern analyses , 2014, NeuroImage.
[30] S. Dehaene,et al. Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words , 1992, Cognition.
[31] S. Dehaene,et al. Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.
[32] Eric-Jan Wagenmakers,et al. An EZ-diffusion model for response time and accuracy , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[33] Daniel Ansari,et al. Qualitatively different coding of symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers in the human brain , 2015, Human brain mapping.
[34] Wim Fias,et al. Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[35] Arava Y. Kallai,et al. A generalized fraction: an entity smaller than one on the mental number line. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] Marie-Pascale Noël,et al. Developmental Changes in the Profiles of Dyscalculia: An Explanation Based on a Double Exact-and-Approximate Number Representation Model , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[37] A. Kleinschmidt,et al. A Supramodal Number Representation in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.
[38] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[39] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line , 1999, Cognition.
[40] G. R. Potts. Information Processing Strategies Used in the Encoding of Linear Orderings. , 1972 .
[41] Craig Leth-Steensen,et al. MODELLING SNARC BY USING POLARITY CODES TO ADJUST DRIFT RATES , 2011 .
[42] J. D. Balakrishnan,et al. Is subitizing a unique numerical ability? , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[43] G. Cumming. Understanding the New Statistics: Effect Sizes, Confidence Intervals, and Meta-Analysis , 2011 .
[44] N. Prins. Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .
[45] P. Gordon. Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.
[46] S. Dehaene,et al. Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[47] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[48] Yang Seok Cho,et al. Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[49] C. Gallistel,et al. Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[50] A. Krajcsi. Numerical distance and size effects dissociate in Indo-Arabic number comparison , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[51] A. T. Welford,et al. THE MEASUREMENT OF SENSORY-MOTOR PERFORMANCE : SURVEY AND REAPPRAISAL OF TWELVE YEARS' PROGRESS , 1960 .
[52] J. Tzelgov,et al. Expanding on the mental number line: zero is perceived as the "smallest". , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[53] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Primed numbers : Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming , 1999 .
[54] Bert De Smedt,et al. Visual Number Beats Abstract Numerical Magnitude: Format-dependent Representation of Arabic Digits and Dot Patterns in Human Parietal Cortex , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[55] Emmy Defever,et al. The Approximate Number System is not Predictive for Symbolic Number Processing in Kindergarteners , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[56] Michael Schneider,et al. Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.
[57] Roger Ratcliff,et al. The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.
[58] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.