The next toolkit

Language is an example of collective behavior, and it is a type of collective behavior for which people are highly adapted. Comparable systems in other creatures in no way approach the level of organization found in human languages. Language provides a canonical example of a complex system: robust, adaptable, and selfassembling. Self-assembly occurs both in the cognitive system (as children bootstrap a complex generative system from limited and varied exposure to its manifestations) and in populations, as people match their language systems to each other, and group themselves into social networks of people who share the same language. Linguistic systems arise within the affordances of physics, biology, and society. Take vowels as an example. The fact that vowels can be well characterized using three formant trajectories (disregarding all resonances of the vocal tract above the third one) derives from several facts. One is that the glottal source spectrum rolls off rapidly, with the result that upper formants are not reliably excited. Another is that the hearing systems of people and all other animals effectively compute a spectral representation of the sound, from which the resonances of the sound source can be estimated. Yet another is that the auditory system is most sensitive right in the region of the first three formants. The confluence of these factors as the phonetic grounding of the vowel map is discussed at more length in Pierrehumbert (2000). Not all combinations of three resonances correspond to possible vowels, of course; the entire threedimensional vowel space is effectively circumscribed by the articulatory range of human beings. Thus, physics and biology define a vowel solid, much as the color vision system defines a color solid. Specific languages preferentially exploit different regions of this solid. For example, the region corresponding to cardinal [u] is not exploited in many dialects of American English. In the American South, the closest correspondant to [u] is much fronter; Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006) report F2 values in the range of 1400–2000Hz for this region. Fronter than cardinal [u], but not as front as French /y/, the Southern AE [u] corresponds to a particular region in the three-dimensional vowel space, much as the color word ‘‘teal’’ corresponds to a particular region of the color solid which may not have a precise label in some other languages. Now, we can also consider the cluster of people who share any given variant of /u/. In a dialect atlas, such as Labov et al. (2006), the areas in which a variant predominates are marked as regions on a map. Of course this

[1]  Jules D. Gliesche Review of Principles of Linguistic Change: Social factors , 2005 .

[2]  Jung­Il Suh,et al.  On the Variable Rules , 1983 .

[3]  Lawrence R. Rabiner,et al.  A tutorial on hidden Markov models and selected applications in speech recognition , 1989, Proc. IEEE.

[4]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .

[5]  J. Dyer,et al.  'We all speak the same round here': Dialect levelling in a Scottish-English community , 2002 .

[6]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[7]  Douglas A. Reynolds,et al.  Person authentication by voice: a need for caution , 2003, INTERSPEECH.

[8]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the `tip-of-the-tongue' phenomenon , 1997, Cognition.

[9]  K. Shockley,et al.  Imitation in shadowing words , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Kim M. Isaac What About Linguistic Diversity? , 2001, Language and Nationality.

[11]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Independence of Input and Output Phonology in Word Processing and Short-Term Memory , 1999 .

[12]  Betty S. Phillips,et al.  Word Frequency and the Actuation of Sound Change , 1984 .

[13]  J. Burgoon,et al.  Interpersonal Adaptation: Dyadic Interaction Patterns , 1995 .

[14]  S. Feldstein,et al.  Changes in Vocal Intensity as a Function of Interspeaker Influence , 1972 .

[15]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  "Schema Abstraction" in a Multiple-Trace Memory Model , 1986 .

[16]  Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al.  Word-specific phonetics , 2001 .

[17]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Phonotactics, Neighborhood Activation, and Lexical Access for Spoken Words , 1999, Brain and Language.

[18]  James Sneed German,et al.  Allophonic reassignment in dialect adaptation , 2005 .

[19]  Ulrich Ammon,et al.  Sociolinguistics: An international handbook of the science of language and society (Project announcement) , 1984, Language in Society.

[20]  Pierre-Yves Oudeyer,et al.  Self-Organization in the Evolution of Speech , 2006, Oxford Studies in the Evolution of Language.

[21]  B. Rapp,et al.  Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. , 2000, Psychological review.

[22]  Stanley Wasserman,et al.  Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications , 1994 .

[23]  Norma C Mendoza-Denton,et al.  ‘Muy Macha’: Gender and ideology in gang‐girls’ discourse about makeup* , 1996 .

[24]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Effects of talker, rate, and amplitude variation on recognition memory for spoken words , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  Andrew Wedel,et al.  Category competition drives contrast maintenance within an exemplar-based production/perception loop , 2004, SIGMORPHON@ACL.

[26]  E. Bard,et al.  Controlling the Intelligibility of Referring Expressions in Dialogue , 2000 .

[27]  J. Pierrehumbert The phonetic grounding of phonology , 2000 .

[28]  William Hallett Ham,et al.  Phonetic and Phonological Aspects of Geminate Timing , 2001 .

[29]  P. Eckert,et al.  New Ways of Analyzing Sound Change , 1993 .

[30]  Stanley Wasserman,et al.  Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications , 1994, Structural analysis in the social sciences.

[31]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[32]  P. Denes,et al.  The speech chain : the physics and biology of spoken language , 1963 .

[33]  E. Clark,et al.  The Child's Path to Spoken Language. , 1994 .

[34]  William Labov,et al.  The Social Stratification of (r) in New York City Department Stores , 1997 .

[35]  P. Jusczyk The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .

[36]  Mary E. Beckman,et al.  Phonetic Interpretation Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI: Interpreting ‘phonetic interpretation’ over the lexicon , 2004 .

[37]  William Labov,et al.  On the uses of variable rules , 1979, Language in Society.

[38]  Jessica Maye,et al.  Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination , 2002, Cognition.

[39]  H. Giles,et al.  Speech Accommodation Theory: The First Decade and Beyond , 1987 .

[40]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Exercises in Rethinking Innateness: A Handbook for Connectionist Simulations , 1997 .

[41]  M. Mead,et al.  Cybernetics , 1953, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[42]  Carol A. Fowler,et al.  Shadowing latency and imitation: the effect of familiarity with the phonetic patterning of English , 2003, J. Phonetics.

[43]  J. Pind The Discovery of Spoken Language, Peter W. Jusczyk (Ed.). MIT Press (1997), ISBN 0 262 10058 4 , 1997 .

[44]  Scott McDonald,et al.  Filled pauses and their status in the mental lexicon , 2001, DiSS.

[45]  W. Labov,et al.  The Atlas Of North American English , 2005 .

[46]  Bart de Boer,et al.  Self-organization in vowel systems , 2000, J. Phonetics.

[47]  J. Pierrehumbert,et al.  The influence of sexual orientation on vowel production. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[48]  Janet B. Pierrehumbert Prosody, intonation, and speech technology , 1993 .

[49]  W. Labov Principles of Linguistic Change: Internal Factors , 1994 .

[50]  J. R. Carbonell,et al.  Speaker authentication and identification: a comparison of spectrographic and auditory presentations of speech material. , 1968, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[51]  Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al.  Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast , 2000 .

[52]  Katya Pertsova Production, perception, and emergent phonotactic patterns: A case of contrastive palatalization (review) , 2007 .

[53]  T. Shallice,et al.  Isolating Cognitive Modules with the Dual-Task Paradigm: Are Speech Perception and Production Separate Processes? , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[54]  Jessica Maye,et al.  Learning Phonemes Without Minimal Pairs , 2000 .

[55]  Sharon Peperkamp,et al.  Learning the mapping from surface to underlying representations in an artificial language , 2006 .

[56]  S. Goldinger Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. , 1998, Psychological review.

[57]  J. Pierrehumbert Phonetic Diversity, Statistical Learning, and Acquisition of Phonology , 2003, Language and speech.

[58]  Biing-Hwang Juang,et al.  Fundamentals of speech recognition , 1993, Prentice Hall signal processing series.

[59]  John C. Paolillo Analyzing Linguistic Variation: Statistical Models and Methods , 2001 .

[60]  J. Stemberger Apparent anti-frequency effects in language production: The Addition Bias and phonological underspecification☆ , 1991 .

[61]  S. Goldinger Words and voices: episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[62]  P. Luce,et al.  When Words Compete: Levels of Processing in Perception of Spoken Words , 1998 .

[63]  David B. Pisoni,et al.  Some acoustic cues for the perceptual categorization of American English regional dialects , 2004, J. Phonetics.

[64]  W. Labov Principles Of Linguistic Change , 1994 .