When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples.

Interactions between levels of learner knowledge in a domain and levels of instructional guidance were investigated. Inexperienced mechanical trade apprentices were presented with either a series of worked examples to study or problems to solve. On subsequent tests, inexperienced trainees benefited most from the worked examples condition, with this group performing better with lower ratings of mental load than similar trainees who solved problems. With more experience in the domain, worked examples became redundant and problem solving proved superior. It is suggested that the relative effectiveness of either worked examples or problem solving depends heavily on levels of learner knowledge.

[1]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[2]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[3]  Paul J. Feltovich,et al.  Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices , 1981, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  N. Moray Subjective Mental Workload , 1982 .

[5]  M. Levine,et al.  Effects of Goal Specificity on Means-Ends Analysis and Learning. , 1982 .

[6]  J. Sweller,et al.  Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving. , 1983 .

[7]  M. Chi,et al.  Problem-Solving Ability. , 1985 .

[8]  J. Sweller,et al.  What do students learn while solving mathematics problems , 1985 .

[9]  J. Sweller,et al.  The Use of Worked Examples as a Substitute for Problem Solving in Learning Algebra , 1985 .

[10]  H. Simon,et al.  Why are some problems hard? Evidence from Tower of Hanoi , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  R. Sternberg Human Abilities: An Information Processing Approach , 1985 .

[12]  F. Thomas Eggemeier,et al.  Workload assessment methodology. , 1986 .

[13]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[14]  H. Simon,et al.  Learning Mathematics From Examples and by Doing , 1987 .

[15]  J. Sweller,et al.  Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. , 1987 .

[16]  R. A. Tarmizi,et al.  Guidance during Mathematical Problem Solving. , 1988 .

[17]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[18]  K. J. J. Gilhooly,et al.  Human and Machine Problem Solving , 1989 .

[19]  Joan K. Gallini,et al.  When Is an Illustration Worth Ten Thousand Words , 1990 .

[20]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive load as a factor in the structuring of technical material. , 1990 .

[21]  J. Sweller,et al.  Structuring Effective Worked Examples , 1990 .

[22]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction , 1991 .

[23]  Fred G. W. C. Paas,et al.  The Efficiency of Instructional Conditions: An Approach to Combine Mental Effort and Performance Measures , 1992 .

[24]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[25]  Nigel H. Goddard,et al.  Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1993 .

[26]  J. Sweller COGNITIVE LOAD THEORY, LEARNING DIFFICULTY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN , 1994 .

[27]  F. Paas,et al.  Variability of Worked Examples and Transfer of Geometrical Problem-Solving Skills: A Cognitive-Load Approach , 1994 .

[28]  William M. Carroll Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. , 1994 .

[29]  R. Mayer,et al.  A generative theory of textbook design: Using annotated illustrations to foster meaningful learning of science text , 1995 .

[30]  Jill L. Quilici,et al.  Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. , 1996 .

[31]  Paul Chandler,et al.  Levels of Expertise and Instructional Design , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[32]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[33]  Richard E. Mayer,et al.  Research-based principles for the design of instructional messages: The case of multimedia explanations , 1999 .

[34]  J. Tuovinen,et al.  A comparison of cognitive load associated with discovery learning and worked examples , 1999 .

[35]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  From Studying Examples to Solving Problem: Fading Worked-Out Solution Steps Helps Learning , 2000 .

[36]  S. Derry,et al.  Learning from Examples: Instructional Principles from the Worked Examples Research , 2000 .

[37]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Incorporating Learner Experience into the Design of Multimedia Instruction. , 2000 .

[38]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Learner Experience and Efficiency of Instructional Guidance , 2001 .

[39]  Richard E. Mayer,et al.  Multimedia Learning , 2001, Visible Learning Guide to Student Achievement.