Probabilistic human intention modeling for cognitive augmentation

The aim of cognitive augmentation is to expand the intrinsically limited human's cognitive abilities caused by cognitive impairment or disability. In order to assist the human's limited cognitive ability, we are trying to develop a human augmented cognition system that aims to provide the appropriate information actively corresponding to what user intents to do. In this paper, we mainly address the probabilistic human intention modeling for cognitive augmentation, and its overall process. The types of implicit intention such as navigational and informational intention can be predicted by using fixation count and length induced by eyeball movement. Also, the gradient of pupil size variation is used to detect the transition point between navigational intent and the informational intent. A Naïve Bayes classifier is used as a tool for the extraction of query keywords to search and retrieve specific information from personalized knowledge database according to the successive series of attended objects according to a specific informational intent in a situation. The experimental results show that the probabilistic human intention model is suitable for achieving the ultimate purpose of the cognitive augmentation.

[1]  U. Schwarz,et al.  Cognitive eyes , 2004 .

[2]  Wei-Ying Ma,et al.  User Intention Modeling in Web Applications Using Data Mining , 2002, World Wide Web.

[3]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Daniel J. Simons,et al.  Current Approaches to Change Blindness , 2000 .

[5]  Gary Berg-Cross,et al.  Intentions and intention recognition in intelligent agents , 2010, PerMIS.

[6]  Sandra P. Marshall,et al.  Integrating psychophysiological measures of cognitive workload and eye movements to detect strategy shifts , 2003, 36th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, 2003. Proceedings of the.

[7]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[8]  Minho Lee,et al.  Recognition of Human's Implicit Intention Based on an Eyeball Movement Pattern Analysis , 2011, ICONIP.

[9]  Pedro M. Domingos,et al.  On the Optimality of the Simple Bayesian Classifier under Zero-One Loss , 1997, Machine Learning.

[10]  B. Goldwater Psychological significance of pupillary movements. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  Uwe D. Hanebeck,et al.  A generic model for estimating user intentions in human-robot cooperation , 2005, ICINCO.

[12]  Harry Zhang,et al.  The Optimality of Naive Bayes , 2004, FLAIRS.

[13]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  Christos Constantinidis,et al.  A Neural Circuit Basis for Spatial Working Memory , 2004, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[15]  Pankoo Kim,et al.  Information Retrieval Techniques to Grasp User Intention in Pervasive Computing Environment , 2011, 2011 Fifth International Conference on Innovative Mobile and Internet Services in Ubiquitous Computing.

[16]  Qi Cheng,et al.  Human intention recognition in Smart Assisted Living Systems using a Hierarchical Hidden Markov Model , 2008, 2008 IEEE International Conference on Automation Science and Engineering.

[17]  W. Levelt,et al.  Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: a quantitative system analysis , 1993 .

[18]  J. Beatty Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982 .

[19]  Wendy D Roth,et al.  The Rashomon Effect , 2002 .

[20]  Roy Stripling,et al.  Postural Control as a Probe for Cognitive State: Exploiting Human Information Processing to Enhance Performance , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..