Recent computational models of color vision demonstrate that it is possible to achieve exact color constancy over a limited range of lights and surfaces described by linear models. The success of these computational models hinges on whether any sizable range of surface spectral reflectances can be described by a linear model with about three parameters. In the first part of this paper, I analyze two large sets of empirical surface spectral reflectances and examine three conjectures concerning constraints on surface reflectance: that empirical surface reflectances fall within a linear model with a small number of parameters, that empirical surface reflectances fall within a linear model composed of band-limited functions with a small number of parameters, and that the shape of the spectral-sensitivity curves of human vision enhance the fit between empirical surface reflectances and a linear model. I conclude that the first and second conjectures hold for the two sets of spectral reflectances analyzed but that the number of parameters required to model the spectral reflectances is five to seven, not three. A reanalysis of the empirical data that takes human visual sensitivity into account gives more promising results. The linear models derived provide excellent fits to the data with as few as three or four parameters, confirming the third conjecture. The results suggest that constraints on possible surface-reflectance functions and the "filtering" properties of the shapes of the spectral-sensitivity curves of photoreceptors can both contribute to color constancy. In the last part of the paper I derive the relation between the number of photoreceptor classes present in vision and the "filtering" properties of each class. The results of this analysis reverse a conclusion reached by Barlow: the "filtering" properties of human photoreceptors are consistent with a trichromatic visual system that is color constant.
[1]
H. Helson.
Fundamental problems in color vision. I. The principle governing changes in hue, saturation, and lightness of non-selective samples in chromatic illumination.
,
1938
.
[2]
D. B. Judd.
Hue Saturation and Lightness of Surface Colors with Chromatic Illumination
,
1940
.
[3]
K. S. Gibson,et al.
Tristimulus Specification of the Munsell Book of Color from Spectrophotometric Measurements
,
1943
.
[4]
D. Slepian,et al.
Prolate spheroidal wave functions, fourier analysis and uncertainty — II
,
1961
.
[5]
D. B. Judd,et al.
Spectral Distribution of Typical Daylight as a Function of Correlated Color Temperature
,
1964
.
[6]
J. Cohen.
Dependency of the spectral reflectance curves of the Munsell color chips
,
1964
.
[7]
W. Stiles,et al.
Counting metameric object-color stimuli using frequency-limited spectral reflectance functions
,
1977
.
[8]
M. H. Brill,et al.
A device performing illuminant-invariant assessment of chromatic relations.
,
1978,
Journal of theoretical biology.
[9]
M. H. Brill,et al.
Further features of the illuminant-invariant trichromatic photosensor.
,
1979,
Journal of theoretical biology.
[10]
G. Buchsbaum.
A spatial processor model for object colour perception
,
1980
.
[11]
H. Barlow.
What causes trichromacy? A theoretical analysis using comb-filtered spectra
,
1982,
Vision Research.
[12]
G. Buchsbaum,et al.
Chromaticity coordinates of frequency-limited functions.
,
1984,
Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[13]
L. Maloney,et al.
Color constancy: a method for recovering surface spectral reflectance.
,
1986,
Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.