A gentle introduction to the dynamic set-point model of human postural control during perturbed stance

Three models of increasing complexity to capture three observed phenomena of postural control are described. The first-order model mimics the increase in standard deviation of sway with eyes closed, but none of the other phenomena. The damped spring model captures both the increase in standard deviation of sway with eyes closed and the change from phase-lag to phase-lead with increasing frequency of oscillation of a visual surround. The final model, the dynamic set-point model, extends the damped spring model by a degree of freedom that reflects the observed slow drift in baseline. Under the assumption of a strong coupling to positional information, the dynamic set-point model predicts a dependence between natural frequency and position coupling constant of the damped spring model. It is shown that this prediction is borne out in data obtained from haptically perturbed stance but not in data obtained from visually perturbed stance.

[1]  M. Turvey,et al.  Common effects of touch and vision on postural parameters , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coupling of fingertip somatosensory information to head and body sway , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  R. Feynman,et al.  The Feynman Lectures on Physics Addison-Wesley Reading , 1963 .

[4]  G. Schöner,et al.  Position and velocity coupling of postural sway to somatosensory drive. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  M. Turvey,et al.  Influences of Body Lean and Vision on Unperturbed Postural Sway , 1997 .

[6]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Self-calibration based on invariant view recognition: Dynamic approach to navigation , 1997, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[7]  H. Lanshammar,et al.  Analysis of postural sway strategies using an inverted pendulum model and force plate data , 1997 .

[8]  P. Mazur On the theory of brownian motion , 1959 .

[9]  A Straube,et al.  Visual stabilization of posture. Physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  I. J. Myung,et al.  The Importance of Complexity in Model Selection. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[11]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene , 1992 .

[12]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[13]  D. Zwillinger Handbook of differential equations , 1990 .

[14]  C. Gardiner Handbook of Stochastic Methods , 1983 .

[15]  V M Zatsiorsky,et al.  Rambling and trembling in quiet standing. , 2000, Motor control.

[16]  Collins,et al.  Pinned polymer model of posture control. , 1995, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[17]  G. Uhlenbeck,et al.  On the Theory of the Brownian Motion II , 1945 .

[18]  William H. Press,et al.  Numerical recipes in C , 2002 .

[19]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[20]  Edward W. Kamen,et al.  Fundamentals of signals and systems using MATLAB , 1997 .

[21]  A. Edwards,et al.  Body sway and vision. , 1946, Journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  V. S. Gurfinkel,et al.  Kinesthetic reference for human orthograde posture , 1995, Neuroscience.

[23]  G. Schöner,et al.  Action-Perception Patterns Emerge From Coupling and Adaptation , 1998 .

[24]  V S Gurfinkel,et al.  Posture as an organizational structure based on a dual process: a formal basis to interpret changes of posture in weightlessness. , 1988, Progress in brain research.