Attentional Selection of Multiple Goal Positions Before Rapid Hand Movement Sequences: An Event-related Potential Study

A dot-probe paradigm was used to provide physiological evidence for the parallel selection of multiple movement goals before rapid hand movement sequences. Participants executed a sequence of manual pointing movements to two out of three possible goal positions. During movement preparation, a task-irrelevant visual transient (a dot probe) was flashed either at one of both movement goals, or at the third, movement-irrelevant location. The results revealed that the N1 component induced by the presentation of the dot was enhanced if the dot was flashed at one of the movement goals, indicating that both target positions were attended before the initialization of the movement sequence. A second experiment showed that movement-irrelevant locations between the movement goals were not attended, suggesting that attention splits into spatially distinct foci.

[1]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Properties of attentional selection during the preparation of sequential saccades , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  The coordination of eye, head, and hand movements in a natural task , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements , 2006, Vision Research.

[4]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  Kenichi Ohki,et al.  Conversion of Working Memory to Motor Sequence in the Monkey Premotor Cortex , 2003, Science.

[6]  R. Andersen,et al.  The posterior parietal cortex: Sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Attention to adjacent and separate positions in space: An electrophysiological analysis , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[9]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visual and oculomotor selection: links, causes and implications for spatial attention , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  A. Allport,et al.  Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .

[11]  G. R Mangun,et al.  Shifting visual attention in space: an electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  Bruno B Averbeck,et al.  Parallel processing of serial movements in prefrontal cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention , 1993 .

[14]  Jun Tanji,et al.  Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead , 1994, Nature.

[15]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  David E. Irwin Robert D. Gordon Eye Movements, Attention and Trans-saccadic Memory , 1998 .

[17]  H. Deubel,et al.  Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping , 2010, Vision Research.

[18]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space and selective attention , 1994 .

[19]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[20]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intention-related activity in the posterior parietal cortex: a review , 2000, Vision Research.

[21]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 51–66 , 1909 .

[22]  W. Wolf,et al.  P31 phosphor persistence at photopic mean luminance level. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[23]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Manual response preparation and saccade programming are linked to attention shifts: ERP evidence for covert attentional orienting and spatially specific modulations of visual processing , 2006, Brain Research.

[24]  R Kawashima,et al.  Topographic representation in human intraparietal sulcus of reaching and saccade , 1996, Neuroreport.

[25]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  C. Colby Action-Oriented Spatial Reference Frames in Cortex , 1998, Neuron.

[27]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The influence of attending to multiple locations on eye movements , 2005, Vision Research.

[28]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Prefrontal Neural Correlates of Memory for Sequences , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  R. Andersen,et al.  Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex , 1997, Nature.

[30]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Allocation of visual attention to spatial locations: Tradeoff functions for event-related brain potentials and detection performance , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[33]  H. Deubel,et al.  Selective Dorsal and Ventral Processing: Evidence for a Common Attentional Mechanism in Reaching and Perception , 1998 .

[34]  O. Neumann Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .

[35]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Allocation of attention in the visual field. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  D. Ballard,et al.  Memory Representations in Natural Tasks , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  S. A. Hillyard,et al.  Sustained division of the attentional spotlight , 2003, Nature.

[40]  Jon Driver,et al.  Cross-Modal Interactions between Audition, Touch, and Vision in Endogenous Spatial Attention: ERP Evidence on Preparatory States and Sensory Modulations , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Tanji,et al.  Neuronal activity in the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas for temporal organization of multiple movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  H. Deubel,et al.  Visual attention during the preparation of bimanual movements , 2008, Vision Research.

[43]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Covert manual response preparation triggers attentional shifts: ERP evidence for the premotor theory of attention , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  G. Mangun Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[46]  R. Andersen,et al.  Dorsal Premotor Neurons Encode the Relative Position of the Hand, Eye, and Goal during Reach Planning , 2006, Neuron.

[47]  Ross Ihaka,et al.  Gentleman R: R: A language for data analysis and graphics , 1996 .

[48]  J. Tanji,et al.  Integration of temporal order and object information in the monkey lateral prefrontal cortex. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[50]  J. Tanji,et al.  Representation of the temporal order of visual objects in the primate lateral prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  J. Tanji,et al.  Activity in the Lateral Prefrontal Cortex Reflects Multiple Steps of Future Events in Action Plans , 2006, Neuron.

[52]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Lateralized EEG components with direction information for the preparation of saccades versus finger movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Spatial gradients of visual attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[54]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[55]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[56]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Neural basis of visual selective attention. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[57]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Look-ahead fixations: anticipatory eye movements in natural tasks , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 67–81 , 1909 .

[59]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: An electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  S. Luck,et al.  The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[61]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[62]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994 .