Be nice if you have to--the neurobiological roots of strategic fairness.

Social norms, such as treating others fairly regardless of kin relations, are essential for the functioning of human societies. Their existence may explain why humans, among all species, show unique patterns of prosocial behaviour. The maintenance of social norms often depends on external enforcement, as in the absence of credible sanctioning mechanisms prosocial behaviour deteriorates quickly. This sanction-dependent prosocial behaviour suggests that humans strategically adapt their behaviour and act selfishly if possible but control selfish impulses if necessary. Recent studies point at the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in controlling selfish impulses. We test whether the DLPFC is indeed involved in the control of selfish impulses as well as the strategic acquisition of this control mechanism. Using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, we provide evidence for the causal role of the right DLPFC in strategic fairness. Because the DLPFC is phylogenetically one of the latest developed neocortical regions, this could explain why complex norm systems exist in humans but not in other social animals.

[1]  H. Gintis Strong reciprocity and human sociality. , 2000, Journal of theoretical biology.

[2]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Evidence for a Common Representation of Decision Values for Dissimilar Goods in Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  M. Mobius,et al.  How Much is a Friend Worth? Directed Altruism and Enforced Reciprocity in Social Networks , 2007 .

[4]  B. Rockenbach,et al.  Egalitarianism in young children , 2008, Nature.

[5]  Mascha van 't Wout,et al.  COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY NEUROREPORT Repetitive , 2005 .

[6]  O. Lidwell Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society , 1961, Nature.

[7]  E. Fehr,et al.  Cooperation and Punishment in Public Goods Experiments , 1999, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[8]  J. List On the Interpretation of Giving in Dictator Games , 2007, Journal of Political Economy.

[9]  David G. Rand,et al.  Spontaneous giving and calculated greed , 2012, Nature.

[10]  M. Tomasello,et al.  What Makes Human Cognition Unique? From Individual to Shared to Collective Intentionality , 2003 .

[11]  J. Andreoni,et al.  Chapter 82 Partners versus Strangers: Random Rematching in Public Goods Experiments , 1998 .

[12]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Diminishing Reciprocal Fairness by Disrupting the Right Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Science.

[13]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[14]  E. Fehr,et al.  Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Daniel C. McNamee,et al.  Category-dependent and category-independent goal-value codes in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Luke J. Chang,et al.  Norms and expectations in social decision-making , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  James Andreoni,et al.  Why free ride?: Strategies and learning in public goods experiments , 1988 .

[18]  Todd L. Cherry,et al.  Hardnose the Dictator , 2002 .

[19]  Andrew Gelman,et al.  General methods for monitoring convergence of iterative simulations , 1998 .

[20]  E. Fehr,et al.  Altruistic punishment in humans , 2002, Nature.

[21]  Herbert Gintis,et al.  The hitchhiker's guide to altruism: gene-culture coevolution, and the internalization of norms. , 2003, Journal of theoretical biology.

[22]  A. Melis,et al.  How is human cooperation different? , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Ernst Fehr,et al.  Human altruism: economic, neural, and evolutionary perspectives , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  E. Ostrom Collective action and the evolution of social norms , 2000, Journal of Economic Perspectives.

[25]  Andrew Thomas,et al.  The BUGS project: Evolution, critique and future directions , 2009, Statistics in medicine.

[26]  Samuel Bowles,et al.  The Origins of Human Cooperation , 2002 .

[27]  Peter Hammerstein,et al.  Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation , 2003 .

[28]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcranial direct current stimulation? , 2009, Brain Stimulation.

[29]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[30]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  The nature of human altruism , 2003, Nature.

[31]  Jordi Brandts,et al.  The strategy versus the direct-response method: a first survey of experimental comparisons , 2011 .

[32]  Xiao-Li Meng,et al.  POSTERIOR PREDICTIVE ASSESSMENT OF MODEL FITNESS VIA REALIZED DISCREPANCIES , 1996 .

[33]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Value Signals in the Prefrontal Cortex Predict Individual Preferences across Reward Categories , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  V. Smith,et al.  Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games , 2000 .

[35]  E. Fehr,et al.  Changing Social Norm Compliance with Noninvasive Brain Stimulation , 2013, Science.

[36]  Mike Mesterton-Gibbons,et al.  Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation , 2004 .

[37]  N. Bardsley Dictator game giving: altruism or artefact? , 2008 .

[38]  H. Sauermann Beiträge zur experimentellen Wirtschaftsforschung , 1967 .

[39]  Colin Camerer Behavioral Game Theory: Experiments in Strategic Interaction , 2003 .

[40]  Joseph Hilbe,et al.  Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models , 2009 .

[41]  A. Riedl,et al.  The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Arthur Schram,et al.  Indirect Punishment and Generosity Toward Strangers , 2009, Science.

[43]  Hilla Peretz,et al.  Ju n 20 03 Schrödinger ’ s Cat : The rules of engagement , 2003 .

[44]  S. Gächter,et al.  The Long-Run Benefits of Punishment , 2008, Science.

[45]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[46]  Rajiv Sethi,et al.  The Evolution of Social Norms in Common Property Resource Use , 1996 .

[47]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update Time Is of the Essence , 2001, Neuron.

[48]  Verena Utikal,et al.  Affect and fairness: Dictator games under cognitive load , 2014 .

[49]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  The Neural Signature of Social Norm Compliance , 2007, Neuron.

[50]  Dino J. Levy,et al.  Comparing Apples and Oranges: Using Reward-Specific and Reward-General Subjective Value Representation in the Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  G. F. Tremblay,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex , 1989, Neurology.

[52]  Robert J. Oxoby,et al.  Mine and yours: Property rights in dictator games , 2008 .

[53]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Social norms and human cooperation , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  C. Plott,et al.  Handbook of Experimental Economics Results , 2008 .

[55]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Shared intentionality. , 2007, Developmental science.

[56]  V. Smith,et al.  Social Distance and Other-Regarding Behavior in Dictator Games: Reply , 1999 .

[57]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  Ernst Fehr,et al.  Disrupting the prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.