Aging and selective sensorimotor strategies in the regulation of upright balance

The effects of aging and repeated exposures on the capability of the center nervous system (CNS) to select pertinent sensory information and resolve sensory conflicts were investigated with virtual reality. Healthy young and older adults maintained quiet stance while immersed in a virtual environment (VE) for 1 hour during which transient visual and/or surface perturbations were randomly presented. Visual perturbations were induced by sudden pitch or roll plane tilts of the VE viewed through a helmet-mounted display, and combined with or without surface perturbations presented in a direction that was either identical or opposite to the visual perturbations. Results showed a profound influence of aging on postural adjustments measured by EMG responses and displacements of the center of pressure (COP) and body's center of mass (COM) in the recovery of upright stance, especially in the presence of sensory conflicts. Older adults relied more on vision as compared to young adults. Aging affects the interaction of the somatosensory and visual systems on the control of equilibrium during standing and the ability of CNS to resolve sensory conflicts. However, even with a one-hour immersion in virtual environment and exposure to sensory conflicts, it is possible for the CNS to recalibrate and adapt to the changes, while improving balance capability in older adults. Thus, preventive and rehabilitation programs targeting postural control in older adults should take into account the possible impairment of sensory organization or sensorimotor integration and include VE training under conditions of sensory conflicts

[1]  E. Todorov,et al.  Virtual Environment Training Improves Motor Performance in Two Patients with Stroke: Case Report , 1999 .

[2]  T Mergner,et al.  Vestibular-neck interaction and transformation of sensory coordinates. , 1997, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[3]  Emily A Keshner,et al.  Postural responses exhibit multisensory dependencies with discordant visual and support surface motion. , 2004, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[4]  Mark S. Redfern,et al.  The influence of dynamic visual cues for postural control in children aged 7–12 years , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Visual contribution to rapid motor responses during postural control , 1978, Brain Research.

[6]  F B Horak,et al.  Control of stance during lateral and anterior/posterior surface translations. , 1998, IEEE transactions on rehabilitation engineering : a publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[7]  Suzanne G. Leveille,et al.  Lower extremity function and subsequent disability: consistency across studies, predictive models, and value of gait speed alone compared with the short physical performance battery. , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[8]  F. Horak,et al.  Components of postural dyscontrol in the elderly: A review , 1989, Neurobiology of Aging.

[9]  J. Fung,et al.  A simple method to estimate force plate inertial components in a moving surface. , 2004, Journal of biomechanics.

[10]  Emily A Keshner,et al.  Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. , 2007, Gait & posture.

[11]  Heidi Sveistrup,et al.  Motor rehabilitation using virtual reality , 2004, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[12]  Mark S. Redfern,et al.  The Potential Use of Virtual Reality in Vestibular Rehabilitation: Preliminary Findings with the BNAVE , 2002 .

[13]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Changes in the sequencing and timing of muscle response coordination associated with develomental transitions in balance abilities , 1992 .

[14]  R. Kenyon,et al.  Using Immersive Technology for Postural Research and Rehabilitation , 2004, Assistive technology : the official journal of RESNA.

[15]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Postural control in the older adult. , 1996, Clinics in geriatric medicine.

[16]  C. Winstein Knowledge of results and motor learning--implications for physical therapy. , 1991, Physical therapy.

[17]  J M Macpherson,et al.  Attributes of quiet stance in the chronic spinal cat. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Patrick J. Loughlin,et al.  Sensory re-weighting in human postural control during moving-scene perturbations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Judith E. Deutsch,et al.  Post-Stroke Rehabilitation with the Rutgers Ankle System: A Case Study , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[20]  F. Horak,et al.  Role of vestibular information in initiation of rapid postural responses , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  F. Horak,et al.  Influence of stimulus parameters on human postural responses. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Joyce Fung,et al.  Postural responses triggered by multidirectional leg lifts and surface tilts , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Marjorie H. Woollacott,et al.  Visual, Vestibular and Somatosensory Contributions to Balance Control in the Older Adult , 1989 .

[24]  J M Macpherson,et al.  Bilateral labyrinthectomy in the cat: effects on the postural response to translation. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Difference between eye closure and visual stabilization in the control of posture in man. , 1982, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[26]  J. Crémieux,et al.  Lateral orientation and stabilization of human stance: static versus dynamic visual cues , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  J M Macpherson,et al.  Weight support and balance during perturbed stance in the chronic spinal cat. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  J. McComas,et al.  Virtual Reality Applications for Prevention, Disability Awareness, and Physical Therapy Rehabilitation in Neurology: Our Recent Work , 2002 .