Young children with a positive reputation to maintain are less likely to cheat.

The present study examined whether having a positive reputation to maintain makes young children less likely to cheat. Cheating was assessed through a temptation resistance paradigm in which participants were instructed not to cheat in a guessing game. Across three studies (total N = 361), preschool-aged participants were randomly assigned to either a reputation condition, in which an experimenter told them that she had learned of their positive reputation from classmates, or to a control condition in which they received no such information. By age 5, children in the reputation condition cheated less often than those in the control condition even though nobody was watching and choosing not to cheat conflicted with their personal interest. These findings are the first to show that informing children that they have a positive reputation to maintain can influence their moral behavior.

[1]  Susan V. McClintic,et al.  Self-evaluation in young children. , 1992, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[2]  Angela D. Evans,et al.  Elementary school children's cheating behavior and its cognitive correlates. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  David Barner,et al.  Children's Sensitivity to Ulterior Motives When Evaluating Prosocial Behavior , 2014, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  Scott A Miller Children's understanding of second-order mental states. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  C. Yeeles,et al.  Children's understanding of the self‐presentational strategies of ingratiation and self‐promotion , 1990 .

[6]  R. Mccall,et al.  The Genetic and Environmental Origins of Learning Abilities and Disabilities in the Early School , 2007, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[7]  R. Banerjee,et al.  Children's reasoning about the self-presentational consequences of apologies and excuses following rule violations. , 2010, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[8]  Children's understanding of modesty in front of peer and adult audiences. , 2007 .

[9]  Gordon P. D. Ingram,et al.  "Princess Alice is watching you": children's belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  Ruth K. Chao,et al.  Chinese and European American cultural models of the self reflected in mothers' childrearing beliefs. , 1995 .

[11]  Angela D. Evans,et al.  Young children can tell strategic lies after committing a transgression. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  E. Markman,et al.  The generic/nongeneric distinction influences how children interpret new information about social others. , 2011, Child development.

[13]  B. H. Pillow,et al.  Children's Understanding of Reputations , 2006, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[14]  M. Norton,et al.  Children develop a veil of fairness. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  P. Aloise-Young,et al.  The development of self-presentation: Self-promotion in 6- to 10-year-old children. , 1993 .

[16]  K. Hwang Face and Favor: The Chinese Power Game , 1987, American Journal of Sociology.

[17]  E. Markman,et al.  Subtle Linguistic Cues Affect Children's Motivation , 2007, Psychological science.

[18]  K. Fischer,et al.  The organisation of Chinese shame concepts? , 2004 .

[19]  G. Walton,et al.  "Helping" versus "being a helper": invoking the self to increase helping in young children. , 2014, Child development.

[20]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Five-Year Olds, but Not Chimpanzees, Attempt to Manage Their Reputations , 2012, PloS one.

[21]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Origins of human cooperation and morality. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  Stephanie M. Carlson,et al.  Development of executive function during childhood , 2016 .

[23]  Benoît Monin,et al.  Moral Self-Licensing: When Being Good Frees Us to Be Bad , 2010 .

[24]  G. Heyman,et al.  Reasoning about the disclosure of success and failure to friends among children in the United States and China. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[25]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Children's Evaluation of Other People's Self-Descriptions. , 2007, Social development.

[26]  K. Lagattuta,et al.  When you shouldn't do what you want to do: young children's understanding of desires, rules, and emotions. , 2005, Child development.

[27]  H. Ross,et al.  Shading the Truth: Self-Serving Biases in Children's Reports of Sibling Conflicts , 2004 .

[28]  R. Banerjee Audience Effects on Self‐Presentation in Childhood , 2002 .

[29]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup members and potential reciprocators. , 2013, Developmental science.

[30]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: relationship to beliefs about goodness. , 1992, Child development.

[31]  C. Dweck,et al.  Helplessness in early childhood: the role of contingent worth. , 1995, Child development.

[32]  R. Banerjee The development of an understanding of modesty , 2000 .

[33]  Alex Shaw,et al.  Young Children Are More Generous When Others Are Aware of Their Actions , 2012, PloS one.

[34]  T. Murdock,et al.  Grade-level differences in the social value of effort: implication for self-presentation tactics of early adolescents. , 1995, Child development.

[35]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Carrot-Eaters and Creature-Believers: The Effects of Lexicalization on Children's Inferences About Social Categories , 1999 .

[36]  R. Lindsay,et al.  Children's Conceptual Knowledge of Lying and Its Relation to Their Actual Behaviors: Implications for Court Competence Examinations , 2002, Law and human behavior.

[37]  V. Fu,et al.  A comparison of child-rearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. , 1990 .

[38]  V. Talwar,et al.  International Journal of BehavioralDevelopment , 2022 .