The Association Between Group Size And Communicational Complexity According To Conceptual Agreement Theory

We model the evolution of concepts, i.e. how members of a social group associate properties to concepts. Our Agent Based Model (ABM) is based on Conceptual Agreement Theory (CAT), which states that individuals can only infer the conceptual state of others when communicating. Through communication agents develop a conceptual structure which is influenced by three variables: the size of the group, the number of possible properties that may describe each concept and the rate at which agents learn. In general, the results show that these three variables non-linearly interact and that the larger the group and number of available properties, and the slower the learning process, the richer the conceptual structure that emerges from agents’ interactions.

[1]  H. Glock Concepts: Where Subjectivism Goes Wrong , 2009, Philosophy.

[2]  S. Fortunato,et al.  Statistical physics of social dynamics , 2007, 0710.3256.

[3]  H. Brownell,et al.  Category differentiation in object recognition: typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  The preferred level of face categorization depends on discriminability , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[7]  B. Latané Dynamic Social Impact: The Creation of Culture by Communication. , 1996 .

[8]  L. Barsalou Flexibility, Structure, and Linguistic Vagary in Concepts: Manifestations of a Compositional System of Perceptual Symbols , 2019, Theories of Memory.

[9]  Katherine A. Rawson,et al.  Category Norms: An Updated and Expanded Version of the Battig and Montague (1969) Norms. , 2004 .

[10]  Michael J. Miller,et al.  Distributional Ratings of Performance: More Evidence for a New Rating Format , 1997 .

[11]  Andrew D. M. Smith,et al.  Establishing Communication Systems without Explicit Meaning Transmission , 2001, ECAL.

[12]  Mark T. Greenberg,et al.  Environmental influences on early language development: The context of social risk , 1990, Development and Psychopathology.

[13]  Dirk D. Steiner,et al.  Distributional ratings of performance: Further examination of a new rating format. , 1993 .

[14]  T. Schelling Micromotives and Macrobehavior , 1978 .

[15]  T. Rogers,et al.  Object categorization: reversals and explanations of the basic-level advantage. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Perceptual simulation in conceptual combination: evidence from property generation. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[18]  Michael N. Jones,et al.  The semantic richness of abstract concepts , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[19]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  P. Converse The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics , 2004 .

[21]  M. Black,et al.  Translations from the philosophical writings of Gottlob Frege , 1953 .

[22]  Enrique Canessa,et al.  The dynamics of social agreement according to Conceptual Agreement Theory , 2014 .

[23]  J. Hampton Polymorphous Concepts in Semantic Memory , 1979 .

[24]  Jennifer L. Berdahl,et al.  The Study of Groups: Past, Present, and Future , 2000 .

[25]  Sergio E. Chaigneau,et al.  Conceptual agreement theory , 2012 .

[26]  Laurent Lehmann,et al.  On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  Robert Axelrod,et al.  Advancing the art of simulation in the social sciences , 1997, Complex..

[28]  Robert Axelrod Advancing the art of simulation in the social sciences , 1997 .

[29]  B. Ross,et al.  The importance of being coherent: Category coherence, cross-classification, and reasoning , 2006 .

[30]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  The instability of graded structure: implications for the nature of concepts , 1987 .

[31]  Tom M. Mitchell,et al.  Quantitative modeling of the neural representation of objects: How semantic feature norms can account for fMRI activation , 2011, NeuroImage.

[32]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  The Dynamical Perspective in Personality and Social Psychology , 2002 .

[33]  G. Frege On Sense and Reference , 1948 .

[34]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things , 2005, Behavior research methods.