A PDP Account of Transitions in Conceptual Development

As children gain knowledge about the world, the organization of their conceptual knowledge becomes increasingly complex, as reflected by the successive emergence of sensitivity to different types of similarity over the course of development. At the start, when judging the similarity of two objects, infants rely on perceptual similarity. By age 3, children often make these judgments based on object co-occurrence, or thematic similarity. Finally, after ages 5-6, children reliably start to prefer taxonomic similarity. Though these phenomena have been well-studied, they are often explained by reference to separate mechanisms that are stipulated to come on-line at specific ages. We present a PDP model that learns from the structure of its environment and exhibits transitions in the relative salience of perceptual, thematic, and taxonomic similarity, as observed empirically, without any changes in its underlying learning mechanism or training environment.

[1]  Z. Estes,et al.  Thematic thinking : the apprehension and consequences of thematic relations , 2011 .

[2]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  The Emergence of Category Representations During Infancy: Are Separate Perceptual and Conceptual Processes Required? , 2000 .

[3]  M. Johnson,et al.  The emergence of perceptual category representations in young infants: a connectionist analysis. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[4]  Daniel Mirman,et al.  A Tale of Two Semantic Systems: Taxonomic and Thematic Knowledge , 2011, CogSci.

[5]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach , 2004 .

[6]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[7]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Conceptual preference for thematic or taxonomic relations: A nonmonotonic age trend from preschool to old age ☆ , 1979 .

[8]  G. Murphy,et al.  Causes of taxonomic sorting by adults: A test of the thematic-to-taxonomic shift , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Neural representations of events arise from temporal community structure , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[11]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Evidence for Representations of Perceptually Similar Natural Categories by 3-Month-Old and 4-Month-Old Infants , 1993, Perception.

[12]  E. Spelke,et al.  Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change , 1994 .

[13]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[15]  Anna V. Fisher,et al.  Processing of perceptual information is more robust than processing of conceptual information in preschool-age children: Evidence from costs of switching , 2011, Cognition.

[16]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[17]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  The Development of Categorization , 2011 .