Phonotactic and Prosodic Effects on Word Segmentation in Infants

This research examines the issue of speech segmentation in 9-month-old infants. Two cues known to carry probabilistic information about word boundaries were investigated: Phonotactic regularity and prosodic pattern. The stimuli used in four head turn preference experiments were bisyllabic CVC.CVC nonwords bearing primary stress in either the first or the second syllable (strong/weak vs. weak/strong). Stimuli also differed with respect to the phonotactic nature of their cross-syllabic C.C cluster. Clusters had either a low probability of occurring at a word juncture in fluent speech and a high probability of occurring inside of words ("within-word" clusters) or a high probability of occurring at a word juncture and a low probability of occurring inside of words ("between-word" clusters). Our results show that (1) 9-month-olds are sensitive to how phonotactic sequences typically align with word boundaries, (2) altering the stress pattern of the stimuli reverses infants' preference for phonotactic cluster types, (3) the prosodic cue to segmentation is more strongly relied upon than the phonotactic cue, and (4) a preference for high-probability between-word phonotactic sequences can be obtained either by placing stress on the second syllable of the stimuli or by inserting a pause between syllables. The implications of these results are discussed in light of an integrated multiple-cue approach to speech segmentation in infancy.

[1]  P. Jusczyk Bootstrapping from the Signal: Some Further Directions , 2001 .

[2]  A. Samuel,et al.  Implications of stress-pattern differences in spoken-word recognition , 2000 .

[3]  S. Mattys,et al.  The perception of primary and secondary stress in English , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants’ sensitivity to allophonic cues for word segmentation , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Mary R. Newsome,et al.  The Beginnings of Word Segmentation in English-Learning Infants , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  P. Luce,et al.  Probabilistic Phonotactics and Neighborhood Activation in Spoken Word Recognition , 1999 .

[7]  Joost Van de Weijer,et al.  Language input for word discovery , 1999 .

[8]  M. Pitt,et al.  Is Compensation for Coarticulation Mediated by the Lexicon , 1998 .

[9]  P. Luce,et al.  When Words Compete: Levels of Processing in Perception of Spoken Words , 1998 .

[10]  Thierry Nazzi,et al.  Language Discrimination by Newborns: Toward an Understanding of the Role of Rhythm , 1998 .

[11]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Learning to Segment Speech Using Multiple Cues: A Connectionist Model , 1998 .

[12]  J. Mehler,et al.  Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  D. Norris,et al.  The Possible-Word Constraint in the Segmentation of Continuous Speech , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' memory for spoken words. , 1997, Science.

[15]  N. Chater,et al.  Bootstrapping Word Boundaries: A Bottom-up Corpus-Based Approach to Speech Segmentation , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  Catharine H. Echols,et al.  The perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults. , 1997 .

[17]  D. Kemmerer,et al.  Phonotactics and Syllable Stress: Implications for the Processing of Spoken Nonsense Words , 1997, Language and speech.

[18]  P. Jusczyk The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .

[19]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[20]  T. A. Cartwright,et al.  Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation , 1996, Cognition.

[21]  J. Morgan A Rhythmic Bias in Preverbal Speech Segmentation , 1996 .

[22]  O. Bohn,et al.  A cross-language comparison of vowel perception in English-learning and German-learning infants. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  A. Woodward,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to word boundaries in fluent speech , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[24]  J. Werker,et al.  Cognitive Influences on Cross-Language Speech Perception in Infancy , 1995 .

[25]  J R Saffran,et al.  Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. , 1995, Child development.

[26]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Do English-Learning Infants use Syllable Weight to Determine Stress? , 1995, Language and speech.

[27]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[28]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to allophonic differences , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Converging measures of speech segmentation in preverbal infants , 1994 .

[30]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. , 1994 .

[31]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language☆ , 1994 .

[32]  J. Werker,et al.  Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  L. Menn,et al.  Phonological development : models, research, implications , 1994 .

[34]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Sensitivity to the Sound Patterns of Native Language Words , 1993 .

[35]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.

[36]  P K Kuhl,et al.  Perceptual strategies in prelingual speech segmentation , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  A. Friederici,et al.  Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech perception , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  R. N. Aslin,et al.  Segmentation of Fluent Speech into Words: Learning Models and the Role of Maternal Input , 1993 .

[39]  A. Levitt The Acquisition of Prosody: Evidence from French- and English-learning Infants* , 1993 .

[40]  J. Mullennix,et al.  Some consequences of stimulus variability on speech processing by 2-month-old infants , 1992, Cognition.

[41]  E. T. Auer,et al.  Syntagmatic and paradigmatic states in spoken word recognition , 1992 .

[42]  A. Cutler,et al.  Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception , 1992 .

[43]  A. Woodward,et al.  Perception of acoustic correlates of major phrasal units by young infants , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  K. Stevens,et al.  Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.

[45]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[46]  A. Levitt,et al.  Evidence for Language-Specific Rhythmic Influences in the Reduplicative Babbling of French-and English-Learning Infants , 1991, Language and speech.

[47]  B. de Boysson-Bardies,et al.  Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages , 1991 .

[48]  P. Kuhl Human adults and human infants show a “perceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  G. Altmann Cognitive models of speech processing , 1991 .

[50]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Constraining models of lexical access: the onset of word recognition , 1991 .

[51]  S E Trehub,et al.  Infants' perception of rhythm: categorization of auditory sequences by temporal structure. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[52]  B. de Boysson-Bardies,et al.  A crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[53]  Janet F. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. , 1988 .

[54]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Context effects in two-month-old infants' perception of labiodental/interdental fricative contrasts. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.

[56]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access , 1988 .

[57]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary , 1987 .

[58]  Kenneth Ward Church,et al.  Phonological parsing and lexical retrieval , 1987, Cognition.

[59]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[60]  B. Morrongiello,et al.  Auditory Temporal Pattern Perception in 6- and 12-Month-Old Infants. , 1984 .

[61]  B. de Boysson-Bardies,et al.  Discernible differences in the babbling of infants according to target language , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[62]  J. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .

[63]  J. Werker,et al.  Developmental changes across childhood in the perception of non-native speech sounds. , 1983, Canadian journal of psychology.

[64]  P. Kuhl Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy , 1983 .

[65]  J D Miller,et al.  Discrimination of auditory target dimensions in the presence or absence of variation in a second dimension by infants , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[66]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Advances in infancy research , 1981 .

[67]  D B Pisoni,et al.  Discrimination of voice onset time by human infants: new findings and implications for the effects of early experience. , 1981, Child development.

[68]  Brian W. Eukel Phonotactic basis for word frequency effects: Implications for lexical distance metrics , 1980 .

[69]  R. Cole,et al.  How are syllables used to recognize words? , 1980, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[70]  P. Kuhl Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[71]  Dennis H. Klatt,et al.  Speech perception: a model of acoustic–phonetic analysis and lexical access , 1979 .

[72]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Perception of a phonetic contrast in multisyllabic utterances by 2-month-old infants , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[73]  S E Trehub,et al.  Infants' perception of temporal grouping in auditory patterns. , 1977, Child development.

[74]  P. Dale,et al.  Discrimination of linguistic stress in early infancy. , 1977, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[75]  L. Demany,et al.  Rhythm perception in early infancy , 1977, Nature.

[76]  S. Trehub The Discrimination of Foreign Speech Contrasts by Infants and Adults. , 1976 .

[77]  L. Streeter Language perception of 2-month-old infants shows effects of both innate mechanisms and experience , 1976, Nature.

[78]  R. Lasky,et al.  VOT discrimination by four to six and a half month old infants from Spanish environments. , 1975, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[79]  Peter D. Eimas,et al.  Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-l] distinction by young infants , 1975 .

[80]  T. Bever,et al.  Sensitivity to Phonological Universals in Children and Adolescents. , 1975 .

[81]  M. Sanders Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .

[82]  Peter D. Eimas,et al.  Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants , 1974 .

[83]  P. Morse The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy. , 1972, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[84]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Speech Perception in Infants , 1971, Science.

[85]  S. Messer,et al.  Implicit phonology in children , 1967 .

[86]  J. Jenkins,et al.  Studies in the Psychological Correlates of the Sound System of American English , 1964 .

[87]  Donald C. Hildum,et al.  Expectancy and the perception of syllables. , 1956 .