Spatial memory-based behaviors for locating sources of odor plumes

BackgroundMany animals must locate odorant point sources during key behaviors such as reproduction, foraging and habitat selection. Cues from such sources are typically distributed as air- or water-borne chemical plumes, characterized by high intermittency due to environmental turbulence and episodically rapid changes in position and orientation during wind or current shifts. Well-known examples of such behaviors include male moths, which have physiological and behavioral specializations for locating the sources of pheromone plumes emitted by females. Male moths and many other plume-following organisms exhibit “counter-turning” behavior, in which they execute a pre-planned sequence of cross-stream movements spanning all or part of an odorant plume, combined with upstream movements towards the source. Despite its ubiquity and ecological importance, theoretical investigation of counter-turning has so far been limited to a small subset of plausible behavioral algorithms based largely on classical biased random walk gradient-climbing or oscillator models.ResultsWe derive a model of plume-tracking behavior that assumes a simple spatially-explicit memory of previous encounters with odorant, an explicit statistical model of uncertainty about the plume’s position and extent, and the ability to improve estimates of plume characteristics over sequential encounters using Bayesian updating. The model implements spatial memory and effective cognitive strategies with minimal neural processing. We show that laboratory flight tracks of Manduca sexta moths are consistent with predictions of our spatial memory-based model. We assess plume-following performance of the spatial memory-based algorithm in terms of success and efficiency metrics, and in the context of “contests” in which the winner is the first among multiple simulated moths to locate the source.ConclusionsEven rudimentary spatial memory can greatly enhance plume-following. In particular, spatial memory can maintain source-seeking success even when plumes are so intermittent that no pheromone is detected in most cross-wind transits. Performance metrics reflect trade-offs between “risk-averse” strategies (wide cross-wind movements, slow upwind advances) that reliably but slowly locate odor sources, and “risk-tolerant” strategies (narrow cross-wind movements, fast upwind advances) that often fail to locate a source but are fast when successful. Success in contests of risk-averse vs. risk-tolerant behaviors varies strongly with the number of competitors, suggesting empirically testable predictions for diverse plume-following taxa. More generally, spatial memory-based models provide tractable, explicit theoretical linkages between sensory biomechanics, neurophysiology and behavior, and ecological and evolutionary dynamics operating at much larger spatio-temporal scales.

[1]  J. Elkinton,et al.  Evaluation of time-average dispersion models for estimating pheromone concentration in a deciduous forest , 1984, Journal of Chemical Ecology.

[2]  M J Weissburg,et al.  The fluid dynamical context of chemosensory behavior. , 2000, The Biological bulletin.

[3]  R. Quinn,et al.  Three-dimensional characterization of the wind-borne pheromone tracking behavior of male hawkmoths, Manduca sexta , 2008, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[4]  T. Baker,et al.  Reiterative responses to single strands of odor promote sustained upwind flight and odor source location by moths. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  Mark A. Willis,et al.  Adaptive Control of Odor-Guided Locomotion: Behavioral Flexibility as an Antidote to Environmental Unpredictability1 , 1996, Adapt. Behav..

[6]  明 大久保,et al.  Diffusion and ecological problems : mathematical models , 1980 .

[7]  Ernst Kramer,et al.  A Tentative Intercausal Nexus and Its Computer Model on Insect Orientation in Windborne Pheromone Plumes , 1997 .

[8]  J. H. Belanger,et al.  Behavioral strategies underlying pheromone-modulated flight in moths: lessons from simulation studies , 1998, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[9]  Jay A. Farrell,et al.  Tracking of Fluid-Advected Odor Plumes: Strategies Inspired by Insect Orientation to Pheromone , 2001, Adapt. Behav..

[10]  E. A. Arbas,et al.  Odor-modulated upwind flight of the sphinx moth, Manduca sexta L. , 1991, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[11]  E. A. Arbas,et al.  Organization of goal-oriented locomotion: pheromone-modulated flight behavior of moths , 1993 .

[12]  T. Baker,et al.  Odour-plume dynamics influence the brain's olfactory code , 2001, Nature.

[13]  J. Kean,et al.  Modelling pheromone anemotaxis for biosecurity surveillance: moth movement patterns reveal a downwind component of anemotaxis. , 2010 .

[14]  Mark A. Willis,et al.  Odor tracking flight of male Manduca sexta moths along plumes of different cross-sectional area , 2013, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[15]  Measured behavioural latency in response to sex-pheromone loss in the large silk moth Antheraea polyphemus. , 1988, The Journal of experimental biology.

[16]  Thomas C. Baker,et al.  A non‐anemotactic mechanism used in pheromone source location by flying moths , 1983 .

[17]  E. A. Arbas,et al.  Active Behavior and Reflexive Responses: Another Perspective on Odor-Modulated Locomotion , 1997 .

[18]  T. Collett,et al.  Spatial Memory in Insect Navigation , 2013, Current Biology.

[19]  J. Hildebrand,et al.  Coincident stimulation with pheromone components improves temporal pattern resolution in central olfactory neurons. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Paul Graham,et al.  Desert Ants Locate Food by Combining High Sensitivity to Food Odors with Extensive Crosswind Runs , 2014, Current Biology.

[21]  J. Hildebrand,et al.  Recent advances in insect olfaction, specifically regarding the morphology and sensory physiology of antennal sensilla of the female sphinx moth Manduca sexta , 2001, Microscopy research and technique.

[22]  A. S. French,et al.  Dynamic properties of antennal responses to pheromone in two moth species. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  S. R. Farkas,et al.  Chemical Trail-Following by Flying Insects: A Mechanism for Orientation to a Distant Odor Source , 1972, Science.

[24]  Ali Marjovi,et al.  Optimal spatial formation of swarm robotic gas sensors in odor plume finding , 2013, Autonomous Robots.

[25]  R. Cardé,et al.  Insect Pheromone Research: New Directions , 1997 .

[26]  J. Kennedy,et al.  Pheromone-Regulated Anemotaxis in Flying Moths , 1974, Science.

[27]  R. Cardé,et al.  Navigational Strategies Used by Insects to Find Distant, Wind-Borne Sources of Odor , 2008, Journal of Chemical Ecology.

[28]  Jiri Janata,et al.  A Multidisciplinary Study of Spatial and Temporal Scales Containing Information in Turbulent Chemical Plume Tracking , 2002 .

[29]  N D Pentcheff,et al.  Odor Plumes and Animal Navigation in Turbulent Water Flow: A Field Study. , 1995, The Biological bulletin.

[30]  T. Baker,et al.  MaleHeliothis virescens maintain upwind flight in response to experimentally pulsed filaments of their sex pheromone (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) , 1992, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[31]  Brian D. Dickman,et al.  Staying the course: chemical signal spatial properties and concentration mediate cross-stream motion in turbulent plumes , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[32]  J. Murlis,et al.  Effects of pheromone plume structure and visual stimuli on the pheromone-modulated upwind flight of male gypsy moths (Lymantria dispar) in a Forest (Lepidoptera: Lymantriidae) , 1994, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[33]  T. Baker,et al.  Manoeuvres used by flying male oriental fruit moths to relocate a sex pheromone plume in an experimentally shifted wind‐field , 1987 .

[34]  A. Ludlow,et al.  An analysis of anemotactic zigzagging flight in male moths stimulated by pheromone , 1978 .

[35]  J. Kennedy Zigzagging and casting as a programmed response to wind‐borne odour: a review , 1983 .

[36]  J. Murlis,et al.  Fine‐scale structure of odour plumes in relation to insect orientation to distant pheromone and other attractant sources , 1981 .

[37]  T. Baker,et al.  Adaptation of antennal neurons in moths is associated with cessation of pheromone-mediated upwind flight. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  A. Ludlow,et al.  Finding of a sex pheromone source by gypsy moths released in the field , 1983, Nature.

[39]  Daniel Grünbaum,et al.  A spatially explicit Bayesian framework for cognitive schooling behaviours , 2012, Interface Focus.

[40]  Paul S. G. Stein Neurons, networks, and motor behavior , 1999 .

[41]  R. Cardé,et al.  Fine-scale structure of pheromone plumes modulates upwind orientation of flying moths , 1994, Nature.

[42]  Ring T. Cardé,et al.  Antennal resolution of pulsed pheromone plumes in three moth species. , 2002, Journal of insect physiology.

[43]  Mark A. Willis,et al.  Effects of intermittent and continuous pheromone stimulation on the flight behaviour of the oriental fruit moth, Grapholita molesta , 1984 .

[44]  Thomas C. Baker,et al.  Field and laboratory electroantennographic measurements of pheromone plume structure correlated with oriental fruit moth behaviour , 1989 .

[45]  R. Cardé,et al.  Spatial and temporal structures of pheromone plumes in fields and forests , 2000 .

[46]  N. Vickers Mechanisms of animal navigation in odor plumes. , 2000, The Biological bulletin.

[47]  Coby Schal,et al.  Pheromone puff trajectory and upwind flight of male gypsy moths in a forest , 1987 .

[48]  Brian D. Dickman,et al.  Getting ahead: context-dependent responses to odorant filaments drive along-stream progress during odor tracking in blue crabs , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[49]  Randall D. Beer,et al.  Biological Neural Networks in Invertebrate Neuroethology and Robotics. Editors: Randall D. Beer, Roy E. Ritzmann, Thomas McKenna (Academic Press, Inc., Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1993) , 1996, SGAR.

[50]  J. Kennedy,et al.  The steering of zigzagging flight by male gypsy moths , 1987, Naturwissenschaften.

[51]  Donald R. Webster,et al.  Chemosensory guidance cues in a turbulent chemical odor plume , 2001 .

[52]  T. Christensen,et al.  Discrimination of sex pheromone blends in the olfactory system of the moth , 1989 .

[53]  M. Willis 4.42 – Odor Plumes and Animal Orientation , 2008 .

[54]  H. Weimerskirch,et al.  Evidence for olfactory search in wandering albatross, Diomedea exulans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[55]  Christer Löfstedt,et al.  How insects sense olfactory patches : the spatial scaling of olfactory information , 2013 .

[56]  R. Cardé,et al.  Influence of plume structure and pheromone concentration on upwind flight of Cadra cautella males , 1995 .

[57]  John K Kruschke,et al.  Bayesian data analysis. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[58]  Yang Wang,et al.  Collective Odor Source Estimation and Search in Time-Variant Airflow Environments Using Mobile Robots , 2011, Sensors.