A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the United States

Ten native Chinese-speaking children and adolescents who immigrated to the United States between ages 5 and 16 were studied for 3 years. The changes in their language preferences, language environments, and proficiency in English, their second language (L2), as well as Chinese, their first language (L1), were measured quantitatively and qualitatively. Participants with arrival ages of 9 or younger switched their language preference from L1 to L2 within the first year, were exposed to a significantly richer L2 than L1 environment, and became more proficient in L2 than in L1. The older participants maintained their preference for L1 across the 3 years, were exposed to a significantly richer L1 than L2 environment, and maintained L1 as the more proficient language. Interactions among L1 proficiency, peer interactions, social abilities, and cultural preferences jointly influenced the dominant language switch or maintenance processes.

[1]  G. Jia The acquisition of the English plural morpheme by native Mandarin Chinese-speaking children. , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[2]  Doris Aaronson,et al.  Long-term language attainment of bilingual immigrants: Predictive variables and language group differences , 2002 .

[3]  J. Altarriba,et al.  Bilingual Language Mixing: Why Do Bilinguals Code-Switch? , 2001 .

[4]  Michelle R. Molis,et al.  On the evidence for maturational constraints in second-language acquisition , 2001 .

[5]  James Emil Flege,et al.  Pronunciation Proficiency in the First and Second Languages of Korean-English Bilinguals. , 2000 .

[6]  Daniel O. Jackson,et al.  Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis , 2000 .

[7]  Catherine E. Snow,et al.  Three Misconceptions About Age and L2 Learning , 2000 .

[8]  Brian McElree,et al.  The time course of conceptual processing in three bilingual populations , 2000 .

[9]  D. S. Vanderboegh Face(t)s of first language loss. , 2000 .

[10]  Arturo E. Hernandez,et al.  Balancing bilinguals: lexical-semantic production and cognitive processing in children learning Spanish and English. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[11]  G. Yeni-Komshian,et al.  Age Constraints on Second-Language Acquisition , 1999 .

[12]  James E. McDermott,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages. , 1999 .

[13]  Kenji Hakuta,et al.  Confounded age: Linguistic and cognitive factors in age differences for second language acquisition. , 1999 .

[14]  R. Clément,et al.  Conceptualizing Willingness to Communicate in a L2: A Situational Model of L2 Confidence and Affiliation , 1998 .

[15]  Paul P.M. Leseman,et al.  Home Literacy: Opportunity, Instruction, Cooperation and Social-Emotional Quality Predicting Early Reading Achievement , 1998 .

[16]  R. Alcalay,et al.  The Impact of the Wellness Guide/Guía on Hispanic Women's Well-Being-Related Knowledge, Efficacy Beliefs, and Behaviors: The Mediating Role of Acculturation , 1997, Health education & behavior : the official publication of the Society for Public Health Education.

[17]  Stephen Krashen,et al.  UNDER ATTACK/THE CASE AGAINST BILINGUAL EDUCATION , 1997 .

[18]  Birgit Harley,et al.  The critical period hypothesis: Where are we now? , 1997 .

[19]  W. Hartup The company they keep: friendships and their developmental significance. , 1996, Child development.

[20]  J. Kupersmidt,et al.  Similarity as the Basis for Children's Friendships: The Roles of Sociometric Status, Aggressive and Withdrawn Behavior, Academic Achievement and Demographic Characteristics , 1995 .

[21]  I. R. MacKay,et al.  Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  J. S. Johnson,et al.  The Effects of Age on the Rate of Learning a Second Language , 1995, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[23]  J. Kroll,et al.  Transfer Between Picture Naming and Translation: A Test of Asymmetries in Bilingual Memory , 1995 .

[24]  V. Sharma On children's mass media communication , 1995 .

[25]  M. Swain Three functions of output in second language learning , 1995 .

[26]  J. Kroll,et al.  Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations , 1994 .

[27]  M. Braine,et al.  The development of categories at the semantics/syntax interface , 1993 .

[28]  S. Krashen The Power Of Reading , 1993 .

[29]  Y. Minoura A sensitive period for the incorporation of a cultural meaning system: A study of Japanese children growing up in the United States. , 1992 .

[30]  J. S. Johnson,et al.  Critical Period Effects in Second Language Acquisition: The Effect of Written versus Auditory Materials on the Assessment of Grammatical Competence† , 1992 .

[31]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Language Development , 2015 .

[32]  Margaret G. McKeown,et al.  The Nature of Vocabulary Acquisition , 1991 .

[33]  W. Elley Acquiring Literacy in a Second Language: The Effect of Book‐Based Programs * , 1991 .

[34]  Lily Wong Fillmore When Learning a Second Language Means Losing the First. , 1991 .

[35]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Tracking the Unique Effects of Print Exposure in Children: Associations with Vocabulary, General Knowledge, and Spelling. , 1991 .

[36]  M. Patkowski,et al.  Age and Accent in a Second Language: A Reply to James Emil Flege , 1990 .

[37]  Robert C. Gardner,et al.  Social psychological perspectives on second language acquisition. , 1990 .

[38]  Emil Flege,et al.  Age and Accent in a Second Language: A Reply , 1990 .

[39]  T. Pica,et al.  Comprehensible Output as an Outcome of Linguistic Demands on the Learner , 1989, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[40]  J. S. Johnson,et al.  Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  F. Grosjean Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person , 1989, Brain and Language.

[42]  신성철,et al.  Principles of Language Learning and Teaching , 1988 .

[43]  M L Rice,et al.  Lessons from television: children's word learning when viewing. , 1988, Child development.

[44]  M. Swain Communicative competence : Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development , 1985 .

[45]  C. M. Scott,et al.  Adverbial connectivity in conversations of children 6 to 12 , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[46]  Ray A. Chesterfield,et al.  The Influence of Teachers and Peers on Second Language Acquisition in Bilingual Preschool Programs , 1983 .

[47]  Donna M. Johnson Natural Language Learning by Design: A Classroom Experiment in Social Interaction and Second Language Acquisition* , 1983 .

[48]  Francis Mangubhai,et al.  The impact of reading on second language learning. , 1983 .

[49]  Roger L. Hewitt White adolescent Creole users and the politics of friendship , 1982 .

[50]  M. Patkowski The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in a second language. , 1980 .

[51]  R. Selman growth of interpersonal understanding , 1980 .

[52]  S. Oyama The Sensitive Period and Comprehension of Speech , 1978 .

[53]  C. Snow,et al.  Age Differences in the Pronunciation of Foreign Sounds , 1977, Language and speech.

[54]  William Damon,et al.  The social world of the child , 1977 .

[55]  S. Oyama A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system , 1976 .

[56]  D. Crockett,et al.  The spreen-benton aphasia tests, normative data as a measure of normal language development , 1975, Brain and Language.

[57]  Catherine E. Snow,et al.  Age Differences in Second Language Acquisition. , 1975 .

[58]  James J. Asher,et al.  The Optimal Age to Learn a Foreign Language , 1969 .

[59]  W. Lambert A Social Psychology of Bilingualism , 1967 .

[60]  W. Lambert,et al.  Motivational variables in second-language acquisition. , 1959, Canadian journal of psychology.

[61]  E. Murra Learning English as a Second Language , 1954 .