Learning and altering behaviours by reinforcement: Neurocognitive differences between children and adults

[1]  W. Brown Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies , 1912, Nature.

[2]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Francisco Barceló,et al.  Attentional set shifting modulates the target P3b Response in the Wisconsin card sorting test , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  R Verleger,et al.  ERP correlates of associative learning. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[6]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[8]  D. Wallace,et al.  TGF-β1 induction of the adenine nucleotide translocator 1 in astrocytes occurs through Smads and Sp1 transcription factors , 2004 .

[9]  Clintin P Davis-Stober,et al.  Development of the time course for processing conflict: an event-related potentials study with 4 year olds and adults , 2004, BMC Neuroscience.

[10]  E. Crone,et al.  Developmental change in feedback processing as reflected by phasic heart rate changes. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[11]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Charting the maturation of the frontal lobe: An electrophysiological strategy , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[12]  M. Packard,et al.  Habit learning in Tourette syndrome: a translational neuroscience approach to a developmental psychopathology. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[13]  V. Russell,et al.  A dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. , 2005, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[14]  R. Barry,et al.  Development of Inhibitory Processing During the Go/NoGo Task: A Behavioral and Event-Related Potenti , 2005 .

[15]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[16]  E. Darcy Burgund,et al.  The development of sustained and transient neural activity , 2006, NeuroImage.

[17]  David Goodman,et al.  Performance Monitoring in the Anterior Cingulate is Not All Error Related: Expectancy Deviation and the Representation of Action-Outcome Associations , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization. , 2007, Developmental science.

[19]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[20]  I. Daum,et al.  Learning‐related changes in reward expectancy are reflected in the feedback‐related negativity , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  Jutta Kray,et al.  Developmental differences in learning and error processing: evidence from ERPs. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[22]  M. Delgado,et al.  How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  Mark de Rooij,et al.  A Three-Year Longitudinal Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Performance Monitoring and Test-Retest Reliability from Childhood to Early Adulthood , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Modulation of the feedback-related negativity by instruction and experience , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Viktor Müller,et al.  Life Span Differences in Electrophysiological Correlates of Monitoring Gains and Losses during Probabilistic Reinforcement Learning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Age, sex and individual differences in punishment sensitivity: factors influencing the feedback-related negativity. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[27]  M. Paule,et al.  The use of an incremental repeated acquisition task to assess learning in children , 2012, Behavioural Processes.

[28]  D. Hämmerer,et al.  Dopaminergic and prefrontal contributions to reward-based learning and outcome monitoring during child development and aging. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[29]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  Feedback-related Brain Potential Activity Complies with Basic Assumptions of Associative Learning Theory , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Brenda R. J. Jansen,et al.  Decomposing developmental differences in probabilistic feedback learning: A combined performance and heart-rate analysis , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[31]  Yael Arbel,et al.  The Good, the Bad, or the Useful? The Examination of the Relationship between the Feedback-related Negativity (FRN) and Long-term Learning Outcomes , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Lars Schwabe,et al.  On the relationship between learning strategy and feedback processing in the weather prediction task—Evidence from event-related potentials , 2013, Neuropsychologia.