Breaking cover: neural responses to slow and fast camouflage-breaking motion

Primates need to detect and recognize camouflaged animals in natural environments. Camouflage-breaking movements are often the only visual cue available to accomplish this. Specifically, sudden movements are often detected before full recognition of the camouflaged animal is made, suggesting that initial processing of motion precedes the recognition of motion-defined contours or shapes. What are the neuronal mechanisms underlying this initial processing of camouflaged motion in the primate visual brain? We investigated this question using intrinsic-signal optical imaging of macaque V1, V2 and V4, along with computer simulations of the neural population responses. We found that camouflaged motion at low speed was processed as a direction signal by both direction- and orientation-selective neurons, whereas at high-speed camouflaged motion was encoded as a motion-streak signal primarily by orientation-selective neurons. No population responses were found to be invariant to the camouflage contours. These results suggest that the initial processing of camouflaged motion at low and high speeds is encoded as direction and motion-streak signals in primate early visual cortices. These processes are consistent with a spatio-temporal filter mechanism that provides for fast processing of motion signals, prior to full recognition of camouflage-breaking animals.

[1]  Wei Wang,et al.  Orientation-Cue Invariant Population Responses to Contrast-Modulated and Phase-Reversed Contour Stimuli in Macaque V1 and V2 , 2014, PloS one.

[2]  Shude Zhu,et al.  An Orientation Map for Motion Boundaries in Macaque V2. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[4]  Rufin Vogels,et al.  Processing of kinetic boundaries in macaque V4. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  D. Ts'o,et al.  Functional organization of primate visual cortex revealed by high resolution optical imaging. , 1990, Science.

[6]  T. Albright,et al.  Neuronal responses to edges defined by luminance vs. temporal texture in macaque area V1 , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[7]  Innes C. Cuthill,et al.  Camouflage, detection and identification of moving targets , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Mark Edwards,et al.  Motion streaks improve motion detection , 2007, Vision Research.

[9]  Graeme D Ruxton,et al.  Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses , 2011, BMC Biology.

[10]  C. Gross,et al.  Visual topography of striate projection zone (MT) in posterior superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  N. Marshall,et al.  To Be Seen or to Hide: Visual Characteristics of Body Patterns for Camouflage and Communication in the Australian Giant Cuttlefish Sepia apama , 2011, The American Naturalist.

[12]  Leo L. Lui,et al.  Single-unit responses to kinetic stimuli in New World monkey area V2: Physiological characteristics of cue-invariant neurones , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Chang'an A Zhan,et al.  Boundary cue invariance in cortical orientation maps. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  S. Zeki,et al.  The Organization of Connections between Areas V5 and V2 in Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex , 1989, The European journal of neuroscience.

[15]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Spatiotemporal Elements of Macaque V1 Receptive Fields , 2005, Neuron.

[16]  G. Orban,et al.  Processing of kinetically defined boundaries in areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[18]  Wei Wang,et al.  Distinct Functional Organizations for Processing Different Motion Signals in V1, V2, and V4 of Macaque , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Strategies for active camouflage of motion , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[21]  Zygmunt Pizlo,et al.  Camouflage and visual perception , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  Wei Wang,et al.  Equivalent Representation of Real and Illusory Contours in Macaque V4 , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  N. McLoughlin,et al.  The Mechanism for Processing Random-Dot Motion at Various Speeds in Early Visual Cortices , 2014, PloS one.

[24]  S. Thorpe,et al.  How parallel is visual processing in the ventral pathway? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  J. Zanker,et al.  Motion camouflage induced by zebra stripes. , 2014, Zoology.

[26]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Theta motion: a paradoxical stimulus to explore higher order motion extraction , 1993, Vision Research.

[27]  Lael J. Schooler,et al.  Are Stripes Beneficial? Dazzle Camouflage Influences Perceived Speed and Hit Rates , 2013, PloS one.

[28]  S. Zeki,et al.  The processing of kinetic contours in the brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Amol Gharat,et al.  Motion-defined contour processing in the early visual cortex. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  R. L. Valois,et al.  The orientation and direction selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex , 1982, Vision Research.

[31]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Orientation selectivity of motion-boundary responses in human visual cortex. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Gregory Francis,et al.  Perceived motion in orientational afterimages: direction and speed , 2001, Vision Research.

[34]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Visual Response Properties of Striate Cortical Neurons Projecting to Area MT in Macaque Monkeys , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  E. Callaway,et al.  Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  Adriane E Seiffert,et al.  Functional MRI studies of human visual motion perception: texture, luminance, attention and after-effects. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  D Jancke,et al.  Orientation Formed by a Spot's Trajectory: A Two-Dimensional Population Approach in Primary Visual Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Hisashi Tanigawa,et al.  A Motion Direction Map in Macaque V2 , 2010, Neuron.

[39]  M. Carandini,et al.  Mapping of stimulus energy in primary visual cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. , 2001, Visual neuroscience.

[41]  Leonard E. White,et al.  Mapping multiple features in the population response of visual cortex , 2003, Nature.

[42]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Interaction between primary and secondary mechanisms in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[43]  John Ross,et al.  Direct Evidence That “Speedlines” Influence Motion Mechanisms , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  D. Burr,et al.  Motion vision: Are ‘speed lines’ used in human visual motion? , 2000, Current Biology.

[45]  G. Orban,et al.  Cue-invariant shape selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. , 1993, Science.

[46]  G. Orban,et al.  Processing of kinetically defined boundaries in the cortical motion area MT of the macaque monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  David Alais,et al.  Temporal Integration of Movement: The Time-Course of Motion Streaks Revealed by Masking , 2011, PloS one.

[48]  G. Sperling,et al.  Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[49]  R. Baddeley,et al.  A review of cuttlefish camouflage and object recognition and evidence for depth perception , 2008, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[50]  R Vogels,et al.  Responses of monkey inferior temporal neurons to luminance-, motion-, and texture-defined gratings. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  M. Hawken,et al.  Laminar organization and contrast sensitivity of direction-selective cells in the striate cortex of the Old World monkey , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Shohet,et al.  Perception of edges and visual texture in the camouflage of the common cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[53]  Leo L. Lui,et al.  Breaking camouflage: responses of neurons in the middle temporal area to stimuli defined by coherent motion , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[54]  Martin Stevens,et al.  Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success , 2014, BMC Evolutionary Biology.

[55]  D. Regan,et al.  Figure-ground segregation by motion contrast and by luminance contrast. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[56]  G. Orban,et al.  Shape selectivity for camouflage-breaking dynamic stimuli in dorsal V4 neurons. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  Tanya I. Baker,et al.  Cortical maps of separable tuning properties predict population responses to complex visual stimuli. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al.  Direct evidence for encoding of motion streaks in human visual cortex , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[59]  S. Merilaita,et al.  Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  Guy A Orban,et al.  Higher order visual processing in macaque extrastriate cortex. , 2008, Physiological reviews.

[61]  Marcello G P Rosa,et al.  Physiological responses of New World monkey V1 neurons to stimuli defined by coherent motion. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  G. Blasdel,et al.  Differential imaging of ocular dominance and orientation selectivity in monkey striate cortex , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.