Dehumanized Perception: A Psychological Means to Facilitate Atrocities, Torture, and Genocide?

Dehumanized perception, a failure to spontaneously consider the mind of another person, may be a psychological mechanism facilitating inhumane acts like torture. Social cognition - considering someone's mind - recognizes the other as a human being subject to moral treatment. Social neuroscience has reliably shown that participants normally activate a social-cognition neural network to pictures and thoughts of other people; our previous work shows that parts of this network uniquely fail to engage for traditionally dehumanized targets (homeless persons or drug addicts; see Harris & Fiske, 2009, for review). This suggests participants may not consider these dehumanized groups' minds. Study 1 demonstrates that participants do fail to spontaneously think about the contents of these targets' minds when imagining a day in their life, and rate them differently on a number of human-perception dimensions. Study 2 shows that these human-perception dimension ratings correlate with activation in brain regions beyond the social-cognition network, including areas implicated in disgust, attention, and cognitive control. These results suggest that disengaging social cognition affects a number of other brain processes and hints at some of the complex psychological mechanisms potentially involved in atrocities against humanity.

[1]  Mina Cikara,et al.  On the wrong side of the trolley track: neural correlates of relative social valuation. , 2010, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  G. Āllport The Nature of Prejudice , 1954 .

[3]  Jeroen Vaes,et al.  Psychological essentialism and the differential attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups , 2001 .

[4]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Regions of the MPFC differentially tuned to social and nonsocial affective evaluation , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  S. Fiske,et al.  Social neuroscience evidence for dehumanised perception , 2009 .

[6]  S. Fiske,et al.  Dehumanizing the Lowest of the Low , 2006, Psychological science.

[7]  Susan V. Opotow Moral Exclusion and Injustice: An Introduction , 1990 .

[8]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[9]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[10]  Daniel Bar-Tal,et al.  Stereotyping and prejudice: Changing conceptions. , 1989 .

[11]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[12]  T. Elbert,et al.  When combat prevents PTSD symptoms—results from a survey with former child soldiers in Northern Uganda , 2012, BMC Psychiatry.

[13]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. , 1988 .

[14]  D. Wegner,et al.  Dimensions of Mind Perception , 2007, Science.

[15]  Susan T. Fiske,et al.  Emotional prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism - The 2002 Tajfel lecture , 2003 .

[16]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  N. Haslam Dehumanization: An Integrative Review , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[18]  R. Dolan,et al.  The neurobiology of punishment , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Ervin Staub,et al.  The roots of evil : the origins of genocide and other group violence , 1990 .

[21]  M. Olff,et al.  European Journal of Psychotraumatology , 2011, European journal of psychotraumatology.

[22]  S. Fiske,et al.  Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[23]  D. Bar-Tal Delegitimization: The Extreme Case of Stereotyping and Prejudice , 1989 .

[24]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  Why Ordinary People Torture Enemy Prisoners , 2004, Science.

[27]  Virginia S. Y. Kwan,et al.  Missing links in social cognition: The continuum from nonhuman agents to dehumanized humans. , 2008 .

[28]  Phillip Atiba Goff,et al.  Not yet human: implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Cortical Regions for Judgments of Emotions and Personality Traits from Point-light Walkers , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  T. Elbert,et al.  The thrill of being violent as an antidote to posttraumatic stress disorder in Rwandese genocide perpetrators , 2011, European journal of psychotraumatology.

[31]  Uta Frith,et al.  The Biological Basis of Social Interaction , 2001 .